Cortical regions recruited for complex active-learning strategies and action planning exhibit rapid reactivation during memory retrieval

Memory retrieval can involve activity in the same sensory cortical regions involved in perception of the original event, and this neural "reactivation" has been suggested as an important mechanism of memory retrieval. However, it is still unclear if fragments of experience other than sensory information are retained and later reactivated during retrieval. For example, learning in non-laboratory settings generally involves active exploration of memoranda, thus requiring the generation of action plans for behavior and the use of strategies deployed to improve subsequent memory performance. Is information pertaining to action planning and strategic processing retained and reactivated during retrieval? To address this question, we compared ERP correlates of memory retrieval for objects that had been studied in an active manner involving action planning and strategic processing to those for objects that had been studied passively. Memory performance was superior for actively studied objects, and unique ERP retrieval correlates for these objects were identified when subjects remembered the specific spatial locations at which objects were studied. Early-onset frontal shifts in ERP correlates of retrieval were noted for these objects, which parallel the recruitment of frontal cortex during learning object locations previously identified using fMRI with the same paradigm. Notably, ERPs during recall for items studied with a specific viewing strategy localized to the same supplementary motor cortex region previously identified with fMRI when this strategy was implemented during study, suggesting rapid reactivation of regions directly involved in strategic action planning. Collectively, these results implicate neural populations involved in learning in important retrieval functions, even for those populations involved in strategic control and action planning. Notably, these episodic features are not generally reported during recollective experiences, suggesting that reactivation is a more general property of memory retrieval that extends beyond those fragments of perceptual information that might be needed to re-live the past.

[1]  J. Gabrieli Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. , 1998, Annual review of psychology.

[2]  K. Paller,et al.  Brain potentials associated with recollective processing of spoken words , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[3]  M. Shapiro,et al.  Representing episodes in the mammalian brain , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system , 1993 .

[5]  Michael Burke,et al.  Cortical Activation Patterns during Long-term Memory Retrieval of Visually or Haptically Encoded Objects and Locations , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  D. Rubin,et al.  The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  A. Senkfor Memory for pantomimed actions versus actions with real objects , 2008, Cortex.

[8]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Nobody Is Perfect: ERP Effects Prior to Performance Errors in Musicians Indicate Fast Monitoring Processes , 2009, PloS one.

[9]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. , 1994, Science.

[10]  J. Gardiner,et al.  Forgetting in recognition memory with and without recollective experience , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[11]  R. Buckner,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience og remembering , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  E. Tulving Memory and consciousness. , 1985 .

[13]  Kara D. Federmeier,et al.  Spontaneous revisitation during visual exploration as a link among strategic behavior, learning, and the hippocampus , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  A. P. R Smith,et al.  fMRI correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts , 2004, NeuroImage.

[15]  D. Schacter Implicit expressions of memory in organic amnesia: learning of new facts and associations. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[16]  J. O’Neill,et al.  Play it again: reactivation of waking experience and memory , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  Jared F. Danker,et al.  The ghosts of brain states past: remembering reactivates the brain regions engaged during encoding. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  L Nyberg,et al.  Activity in motor areas while remembering action events , 2000, Neuroreport.

[19]  Frank Rösler,et al.  Event-related Brain Potentials During Recognition of Ordinary and Bizarre Action Phrases Following Verbal and Subject-performed Encoding Conditions , 1999 .

[20]  F. Rösler,et al.  Material-specific long-term memory representations of faces and spatial positions: Evidence from slow event-related brain potentials , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  JamesW. Lewis Cortical Networks Related to Human Use of Tools , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[22]  M. Wilson,et al.  Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  T. Nurmikko,et al.  Retrieving autobiographical memories of painful events activates the anterior cingulate cortex and inferior frontal gyrus. , 2007, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[24]  Michael Burke,et al.  Content-specific activation during associative long-term memory retrieval , 2005, NeuroImage.

[25]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[26]  Marta Kutas,et al.  Episodic Action Memory for Real Objects: An ERP Investigation With Perform, Watch, and Imagine Action Encoding Tasks Versus a Non-Action Encoding Task , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  S. Wise The primate premotor cortex: past, present, and preparatory. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  B. Underwood Attributes of Memory , 1983 .

[30]  E. Lettich,et al.  Modified Nomenclature for the “10%” Electrode System1 , 1988 .

[31]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[32]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  Reactivation of Motor Brain Areas during Explicit Memory for Actions , 2001, NeuroImage.

[33]  Joel L. Voss,et al.  Time to Go Our Separate Ways: Opposite Effects of Study Duration on Priming and Recognition Reveal Distinct Neural Substrates , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[34]  L. Davachi,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Functional–neuroanatomic Correlates of Recollection: Implications for Models of Recognition Memory , 2022 .

[35]  A. Damasio Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[36]  B. Rossion,et al.  Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart's Object Pictorial Set: The Role of Surface Detail in Basic-Level Object Recognition , 2004, Perception.

[37]  W. Schneider,et al.  Perceptual Knowledge Retrieval Activates Sensory Brain Regions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Neal J. Cohen,et al.  Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitional exploratory behavior enhances learning , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Michael D Rugg,et al.  Remembrance of Odors Past Human Olfactory Cortex in Cross-Modal Recognition Memory , 2004, Neuron.

[40]  J K Adamowicz,et al.  Verbalization, cognitive effort, and heart rate alteration. , 1972, Canadian journal of psychology.

[41]  Jeffrey D. Johnson,et al.  Recollection and the reinstatement of encoding-related cortical activity. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  H. Foley,et al.  Source-monitoring judgments about anagrams and their solutions: Evidence for the role of cognitive operations information in memory , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[43]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[44]  C. A. Castro,et al.  Spatial selectivity of rat hippocampal neurons: dependence on preparedness for movement. , 1989, Science.

[45]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural Systems Shared by Visual Imagery and Visual Perception: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1997, NeuroImage.

[46]  M. Conway,et al.  The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. , 2000, Psychological review.

[47]  Young Ho Kim,et al.  Role of active movement in place‐specific firing of hippocampal neurons , 2005, Hippocampus.

[48]  Albert K. Lee,et al.  Memory of Sequential Experience in the Hippocampus during Slow Wave Sleep , 2002, Neuron.

[49]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex predicts and evaluates the timing of action outcomes , 2011, NeuroImage.

[50]  H. Spiers,et al.  Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[51]  N. A. Bernstein Dexterity and Its Development , 1996 .

[52]  Marta Kutas,et al.  The intractability of scaling scalp distributions to infer neuroelectric sources. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[53]  K. Paller,et al.  Consolidating dispersed neocortical memories: the missing link in amnesia. , 1997, Memory.