Testosterone: Activation or organization of spatial cognition?

Previous studies in animals have revealed effects of both prenatal and current testosterone on brain organization and behavior. However, it is unclear how these effects translate to the human brain. Here, we refine previous procedures to clarify the relative contribution of prenatal versus current testosterone indices to cognitive function. Sixty-nine subjects performed four computerized tasks measuring mental rotation, targeting, figure-disembedding and perceptual discrimination. Using stepwise regression analyses, performance was related to free testosterone assayed in saliva samples and to second-to-fourth finger length ratios (2D:4D), a putative index of prenatal testosterone exposure. The exclusive predictor for mental rotation was found to be sex, while 2D:4D was found to be the sole predictor of targeting, exhibiting a curvilinear relation, and figure-disembedding performance, showing a linear relation. These findings suggest a substantial role for prenatal testosterone but not current testosterone in determining cognitive performance.

[1]  M. Bryden,et al.  Sex Differences and the Role of Figural Complexity in Determining the Rate of Mental Rotation , 1990, Perceptual and Motor Skills.

[2]  Raymond H. Myers Classical and modern regression with applications , 1986 .

[3]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[4]  C. Acerini,et al.  In-utero androgen exposure and 2nd to 4th digit length ratio-comparisons between healthy controls and females with classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia. , 2003, Human reproduction.

[5]  M. Hines,et al.  Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia , 2003, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[6]  Rozmin Halari,et al.  Sex differences and individual differences in cognitive performance and their relationship to endogenous gonadal hormones and gonadotropins. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  Jon Driver,et al.  Reappraising the apparent costs of attending to two separate visual objects , 2000, Vision Research.

[8]  Onur Güntürkün,et al.  Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. , 2000 .

[9]  L. Nolan,et al.  Biological psychology , 2019, An Introduction to the Psychology of Humor.

[10]  J. Watson,et al.  Preschool Embedded Figures Test Performance of Young Children: Age and Gender Differences , 1993, Perceptual and motor skills.

[11]  K. Plaisted Reduced Generalization in Autism: An Alternative to Weak Central Coherence , 2001 .

[12]  Martin Reuter,et al.  Second-to-fourth digit length, testosterone and spatial ability , 2005 .

[13]  G. Wilson,et al.  Biosocial factors, sexual orientation and neurocognitive functioning , 2004, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[14]  Effects of Age and Sex on Speed of Finding Embedded Figures , 1985 .

[15]  M. Konner,et al.  Testosterone levels change with subsistence hunting effort in !Kung San Men , 1987, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[16]  J T Manning,et al.  The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. , 1998, Human reproduction.

[17]  W. Babler,et al.  Early prenatal attainment of adult metacarpal-phalangeal rankings and proportions. , 1975, American journal of physical anthropology.

[18]  J. Manning,et al.  Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: implications for sexual selection in humans. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[19]  R. Deyo,et al.  ANDROGENS AND SPATIAL ABILITY: FAILURE TO FIND A RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TESTOSTERONE AND ABILITY MEASURES , 1987 .

[20]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[21]  F. Brady,et al.  Sports skill classification, gender, and perceptual style. , 1995, Perceptual and motor skills.

[22]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .

[23]  Albert Postma,et al.  Effects of testosterone administration on selective aspects of object-location memory in healthy young women , 2000, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[24]  D. Halpern Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities , 1986 .

[25]  G. Alexander,et al.  Androgen–Behavior Correlations in Hypogonadal Men and Eugonadal Men , 1997, Hormones and Behavior.

[26]  Children's perceptions of internal-external control in intellectual-academic situations and their Embedded Figures Test performance. , 1972, Child development.

[27]  D. Lohman,et al.  The effect of speed-accuracy tradeoff on sex differences in mental rotation , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  K. Christiansen Sex hormone-related variations of cognitive performance in !Kung San hunter-gatherers of Namibia. , 1993, Neuropsychobiology.

[29]  D. Sengelaub,et al.  Prenatal Gonadal Steroids Affect Adult Spatial Behavior, CA1 and CA3 Pyramidal Cell Morphology in Rats , 1998, Hormones and Behavior.

[30]  M. Aldenderfer,et al.  Cluster Analysis. Sage University Paper Series On Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences 07-044 , 1984 .

[31]  L. Hubert,et al.  The relationship between androgen levels and human spatial abilities , 1983 .

[32]  Rebecca C. Knickmeyer,et al.  2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. , 2004, Early human development.

[33]  S. Menard Applied Logistic Regression Analysis , 1996 .

[34]  M. J. Allen,et al.  Strength of Association between Sex and Field Dependence , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[35]  Andrew D. Huberman,et al.  Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation , 2000, Nature.

[36]  Hans C. Jessen,et al.  Applied Logistic Regression Analysis , 1996 .

[37]  M. Bryden,et al.  An investigation of sex differences in spatial ability: mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. , 1977, Canadian journal of psychology.

[38]  H. A. Witkin A Cognitive-Style Approach to Cross-Cultural Research , 1967 .

[39]  Amelia R Hunt,et al.  Eliminating the cost of task set reconfiguration , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[40]  K. Christiansen,et al.  Sex hormones and cognitive functioning in men. , 1987, Neuropsychobiology.

[41]  D. Voyer,et al.  Scoring procedure, performance factors, and magnitude of sex differences in spatial performance. , 1997, The American journal of psychology.

[42]  James W. Pellegrino,et al.  The locus of sex differences in spatial ability , 1979 .

[43]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[44]  D. Duboule,et al.  Of fingers, toes and penises , 1997, Nature.

[45]  Stephen M Kosslyn,et al.  The relationship of male testosterone to components of mental rotation , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Age and sex differences in responses to Embedded Figures and Reversible Figures. , 1969, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[47]  Doreen Kimura,et al.  Nontrivial sex differences in throwing and intercepting: Relation to psychometrically-defined spatial functions , 1991 .

[48]  D. Kimura,et al.  The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns , 1991, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[49]  M. Gauvain Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities.Third Edition. ByDiane F Halpern. Mahwah (New Jersey): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. $79.95 (hardcover); $36.00 (paper). xviii + 420 p; ill.; author and subject indexes. ISBN: 0–8058–2791–9 (hc); 0–8058–2792–7 (pb). 2000. , 2002 .

[50]  D. Ostatníková,et al.  The circalunar cycle of salivary testosterone and the visual-spatial performance. , 2002, Bratislavske lekarske listy.

[51]  A. Aleman,et al.  A single administration of testosterone improves visuospatial ability in young women , 2004, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[52]  S. Stone,et al.  Gonadal hormones and cognitive performance , 1978 .