A theory of the detection and learning of structured representations of similarity and relative magnitude

Responding to similarity, difference, and relative magnitude is ubiquitous in the animal kingdom. However, humans seem unique in the ability to represent relative magnitude and similarity as abstract relations that take arguments (e.g., greater-than (x,y)). While many models use structured relational representations of magnitude and similarity, little progress has been made on how these representations arise. Models that use these representations assume access to computations of similarity and magnitude a priori. We detail a mechanism for producing invariant responses to “same”, “different”, “more”, and “less” which can be exploited to compute similarity and magnitude as an evaluation operator. Using DORA (Doumas, Hummel, & Sandhofer, 2008), these invariant responses can serve to learn structured relational representations of relative magnitude and similarity from pixel images of simple shapes.

[1]  Brian Falkenhainer,et al.  The Structure-Mapping Engine: Algorithm and Examples , 1989, Artif. Intell..

[2]  Charles Kemp,et al.  Exploring the conceptual universe. , 2012, Psychological review.

[3]  Leonidas A A Doumas,et al.  A theory of the discovery and predication of relational concepts. , 2008, Psychological review.

[4]  Dawn Chen,et al.  Bayesian analogy with relational transformations. , 2012, Psychological review.

[5]  K. Holyoak,et al.  The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning , 2012 .

[6]  J. Hummel,et al.  Comparison and Mapping Facilitate Relation Discovery and Predication , 2013, PloS one.

[7]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Structured statistical models of inductive reasoning. , 2009, Psychological review.

[8]  K. Holyoak Analogy and Relational Reasoning , 2012 .

[9]  K. Holyoak,et al.  A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization. , 2003, Psychological review.

[10]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Effects of fictive reward on rat's choice behavior , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[11]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Human-level concept learning through probabilistic program induction , 2015, Science.

[12]  Adrian T. Lee,et al.  fMRI of human visual cortex , 1994, Nature.

[13]  E. Wasserman,et al.  Effects of number of items on the pigeon's discrimination of same from different visual displays. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[14]  J. Hummel,et al.  Modeling Human Mental Representations: What Works, What doesn't and Why? , 2005 .

[15]  D. Gentner,et al.  Respects for similarity , 1993 .

[16]  Frank Tong,et al.  Foundations of Vision , 2018 .

[17]  Christoph von der Malsburg,et al.  The What and Why of Binding The Modeler’s Perspective , 1999, Neuron.

[18]  Miriam Bassok,et al.  OBJECT-BASED REPRESENTATIONS : TRANSFER BETWEEN CASES OF CONTINUOUS AND DISCRETE MODELS OF CHANGE , 1995 .

[19]  S. Phillips,et al.  Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. , 1998, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[20]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Schema induction and analogical transfer , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  John E. Hummel,et al.  Distributed representations of structure: A theory of analogical access and mapping. , 1997 .

[22]  K. Holyoak,et al.  The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning , 2005 .

[23]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  The role of image size and retinal motion in the computation of absolute distance by the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus) , 1990, Vision Research.