Environment size and the use of feature and geometric cues for reorientation.

We tested associative-based accounts of orientation by investigating the influence of environment size on the use of feature and geometric cues for reorientation. Two groups of participants were trained in dynamic three-dimensional virtual rectangular environments that differed in size to find a distinctly colored bin located at one of the four corners. Subsequently, we probed the reliance on feature and geometric cues for reorientation during test trials by presenting six trial types: Small Geometry Only, Large Geometry Only, Small Cue Conflict, Large Cue Conflict, Small Distal, and Large Distal. During Geometry Only test trials, all bins were black; thus, all distinctive featural information was removed leaving only geometric cues. For Cue Conflict test trials, all colored bins were shifted counter-clockwise one corner; thus, the geometric cues from the trained corner and the trained color were in direct conflict. During Distal test trials, the bin in the geometrically incorrect corner farthest from the trained corner was colored the same as during training; the remaining three bins were black. Thus, only this distant feature cue could be used to determine the location of the goal bin. Results suggested that geometric cues were used across changes in environment size, featural cues exerted greater influence when in conflict with geometric cues, and the far featural cue was used to disambiguate the correct from the rotationally equivalent location. In short, both feature and geometric cues were used for reorientation, and environment size influenced the relative use of feature and geometric cues. Collectively, our results provide evidence against associative-based accounts of orientation.

[1]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Importance of Proving the Null , 2022 .

[2]  Nora S Newcombe,et al.  Reorienting When Cues Conflict , 2008, Psychological science.

[3]  Christian F. Doeller,et al.  Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Jeremie Jozefowiez,et al.  Integration of spatial relationships and temporal relationships in humans , 2010, Learning & behavior.

[5]  V. D. Chamizo Acquisition of Knowledge about Spatial Location: Assessing the Generality of the Mechanism of Learning , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  M Zanforlin,et al.  Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). , 1990, Journal of comparative psychology.

[7]  Walter F. Bischof,et al.  Orienting in virtual environments: How are surface features and environmental geometry weighted in an orientation task? , 2008, Cognition.

[8]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[9]  Katherine A. Gaskin,et al.  Overtraining and the use of feature and geometric cues for reorientation , 2013, Psychological research.

[10]  Jean Choi,et al.  Sex-Specific Relationships Between Route-Learning Strategies and Abilities in a Large-Scale Environment , 2006 .

[11]  Debbie M. Kelly,et al.  Encoding of relative enclosure size in a dynamic three-dimensional virtual environment by humans , 2009, Behavioural Processes.

[12]  Kevin G. F. Thomas,et al.  Place Learning in Virtual Space I: Acquisition, Overshadowing, and Transfer , 1997 .

[13]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.

[14]  Antoine Wystrach,et al.  Ants Learn Geometry and Features , 2009, Current Biology.

[15]  Michela Ponticorvo,et al.  Encoding geometric and non-geometric information: a study with evolved agents , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[16]  Marcia L. Spetch,et al.  Blocking in landmark-based search in honeybees , 2001 .

[17]  Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al.  Spatial reorientation: the effects of space size on the encoding of landmark and geometry information , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[18]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[19]  Michael R. W. Dawson,et al.  Using perceptrons to explore the reorientation task , 2010, Cognition.

[20]  Debbie M. Kelly,et al.  Pigeons' (Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. , 1998 .

[21]  J. Prados Blocking and overshadowing in human geometry learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[22]  Sang Ah Lee,et al.  Two systems of spatial representation underlying navigation , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  A. A. Artigas,et al.  Human overshadowing in a virtual pool: Simple guidance is a good competitor against locale learning , 2003 .

[24]  Debbie M. Kelly,et al.  Evidence against integration of spatial maps in humans: generality across real and virtual environments , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[25]  J. Prados,et al.  Spatial integration in human geometry learning , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[26]  S. Gouteux,et al.  Rhesus monkeys use geometric and non geometric during a reorientation task , 2001 .

[27]  Bradley R. Sturz,et al.  Neither by global nor local cues alone: evidence for a unified orientation process , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[28]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[30]  Debbie M Kelly,et al.  Reorienting in images of a three-dimensional environment. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  M L Spetch,et al.  Pigeons encode relative geometry. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[32]  Aaron P Blaisdell,et al.  Sensory preconditioning in spatial learning using a touch screen task in pigeons. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[33]  M L Spetch,et al.  Overshadowing in landmark learning: touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[34]  N. Burgess,et al.  Geometric determinants of human spatial memory , 2004, Cognition.

[35]  Sang Ah Lee,et al.  Beyond Core Knowledge: Natural Geometry , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[36]  J. Rieser,et al.  Bayesian integration of spatial information. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al.  Reorientation by geometric and landmark information in environments of different size. , 2005, Developmental science.

[38]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Explaining the Development of Spatial Reorientation , 2007 .

[39]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Geometry, features and orientation in vertebrate animals : a pictorial review , 2006 .

[40]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  Debbie M. Kelly,et al.  Facilitation of learning spatial relations among locations by visual cues: generality across spatial configurations , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[42]  T. Gurley,et al.  Orientation in trapezoid-shaped enclosures: implications for theoretical accounts of geometry learning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[43]  Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al.  How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[44]  Noam Miller,et al.  Modeling the effects of enclosure size on geometry learning , 2009, Behavioural Processes.

[45]  Valeria Anna Sovrano,et al.  Modularity as a fish (Xenotoca eiseni) views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[46]  S. Shettleworth,et al.  Learning about Environmental Geometry: an Associative Model , 2022 .

[47]  Michael Brown,et al.  Facilitation of learning spatial relations among locations by visual cues: Implications for theoretical accounts of spatial learning , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Whither geometry? Troubles of the geometric module , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  Stuart P. Wilson,et al.  Blocking of goal-location learning based on shape. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[50]  P. Wilson,et al.  Blocking of spatial learning between enclosure geometry and a local landmark. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[51]  J. M. Dabbs,et al.  Spatial Ability, Navigation Strategy, and Geographic Knowledge Among Men and Women , 1998 .

[52]  V. D. Chamizo,et al.  Blocking in the spatial domain. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[53]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.