Systematic bias in traumatic brain injury outcome studies because of loss to follow-up.

OBJECTIVE To identify potential sources of selection bias created by subjects lost to follow-up in studies of traumatic brain injury (TBI). DESIGN Demographic, premorbid, injury-related, and hospital course characteristics were compared for subjects lost and found for 1- and 2-year postinjury follow-ups by using bivariate tests and logistic regression analysis. SETTING Three prospective, longitudinal data sets-a single center, a multicenter, and a statewide incidence surveillance system and follow-up registry. PARTICIPANTS Adolescents and adults hospitalized with a diagnosis of TBI. INTERVENTIONS Not applicable. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Subjects were considered lost when no information was collected from the person with TBI or only limited information could be obtained from a proxy, for any reason, including death, refusal, inability to locate, and inability to interview. RESULTS At year 1 follow-up, 58.0% to 58.6% of subjects were found; 39.7% to 42.0% of subjects were found by year 2. Variables most frequently associated with loss to follow-up were cause of injury, blood alcohol level, motor function, hospital payer source, and race and ethnicity. CONCLUSIONS TBI follow-up studies may experience selective attrition of subjects who (1) are socioeconomically disadvantaged, (2) have a history of substance abuse, and (3) have violent injury etiologies. These phenomena are mitigated for those with more severe motor deficits. Loss to follow-up may be a problem inherent to this population; however, the high rate and its selective nature are problematic for outcome studies.

[1]  A. Edwards,et al.  Outcome studies of brief alcohol intervention in general practice: the problem of lost subjects. , 1997, Addiction.

[2]  S Greenland,et al.  Response and follow-up bias in cohort studies. , 1977, American journal of epidemiology.

[3]  D. Offord,et al.  Ontario Child Health Study follow-up: evaluation of sample loss. , 1991, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[4]  W. Yoels,et al.  Explaining quality of life for persons with traumatic brain injuries 2 years after injury. , 1995, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[5]  C. Granger,et al.  Validity of the functional independence measure for persons with traumatic brain injury. , 1997, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[6]  L. Fugate,et al.  Systematic bias in outcome studies of persons with traumatic brain injury. , 1997, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[7]  S. Baker,et al.  The injury severity score: an update. , 1976, The Journal of trauma.

[8]  C. Harrison-Felix,et al.  Impact of Minority Status on Functional Outcome and Community Integration Following Traumatic Brain Injury , 1996 .

[9]  M Rappaport,et al.  Disability rating scale for severe head trauma: coma to community. , 1982, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[10]  L L Kupper,et al.  Selection bias in epidemiologic studies. , 1981, American journal of epidemiology.

[11]  H. Levin,et al.  Neurobehavioral outcome 1 year after severe head injury. Experience of the Traumatic Coma Data Bank. , 1990, Journal of neurosurgery.

[12]  Eric R. Dahmer,et al.  A model systems database for traumatic brain injury. , 1993 .

[13]  J. Olver,et al.  Prediction of employment status 2 years after traumatic brain injury. , 1995, Brain injury.

[14]  S L Stover,et al.  History, implementation, and current status of the National Spinal Cord Injury Database. , 1999, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[15]  M. Rutter,et al.  Bias resulting from missing information: some epidemiological findings. , 1977, British journal of preventive & social medicine.

[16]  K. Hall,et al.  Descriptive Findings from the Traumatic Brain Injury Model Systems National Data Base , 1996 .

[17]  B. Jennett,et al.  Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. , 1974, Lancet.

[18]  Carl V. Granger,et al.  Advances in functional assessment for medical rehabilitation , 1986 .

[19]  M. Criqui Response bias and risk ratios in epidemiologic studies. , 1979, American journal of epidemiology.

[20]  D. Shern,et al.  Minimizing attrition in longitudinal studies of special populations: An integrated management approach , 1996 .

[21]  D. Gerstein,et al.  Nonresponse and Selection Bias in Treatment Follow-up Studies , 2000, Substance use & misuse.