Visuokinesthetic Realignment in a Video-Controlled Reaching Task
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Pélisson,et al. From Eye to Hand: Planning Goal-directed Movements , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[2] Yann Coello,et al. Determination of target distance in a structured environment: Selection of visual information for action , 2000 .
[3] S. Kitazawa,et al. Prism Adaptation of Reaching Movements: Specificity for the Velocity of Reaching , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] B Wallace,et al. Prism Exposure Aftereffects and Direct Effects for Different Movement and Feedback Times , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[5] Dottie M. Clower,et al. Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[6] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms , 2005 .
[7] Yann Coello,et al. Frame of reference and adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task , 2002, Ergonomics.
[8] Robert B. Welch,et al. Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implications for models of adaptation. , 1994 .
[9] Wilkinson Da,et al. Visual-motor control loop: a linear system? , 1971 .
[10] B Wallace,et al. Effects on prism adaptation of duration and timing of visual feedback during pointing. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[11] T F Shipley,et al. Prism adaptation to dynamic events , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[12] Gordon M. Redding,et al. Chapter 5 Adaptive Eye-Hand Coordination: Implications of Prism Adaptation for Perceptual-Motor Organization , 1992 .
[13] Gordon M. Redding,et al. Adaptive Spatial Alignment , 1997 .
[14] Gordon M. Redding. Decay of visual adaptation to tilt and displacement , 1975 .
[15] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[16] M. Jeannerod,et al. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 1991, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Yves Rossetti,et al. Prismatic displacement of vision induces transient changes in the timing of eye-hand coordination , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[18] Components of displacement adaptation in acquisition and decay as a function of hand and hall exposure , 1976 .
[19] C. Prablanc,et al. Pointing movement in an artificial perturbing inertial field: A prospective paradigm for motor control study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[20] B Wallace,et al. Prism Adaptation During Target Pointing From Visible and Nonvisible Starting Locations. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.
[21] C Ghez,et al. Learning of scaling factors and reference axes for reaching movements. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[22] H. Imamizu,et al. The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] G. M. Redding,et al. Sources of “overadditivity” in prism adaptation , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] R B Welch,et al. Multiple concurrent visual-motor mappings: implications for models of adaptation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] R B Welch,et al. Evidence for a three-component model of prism adaptation. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.
[26] Carole Ferrel,et al. Contrôle sensori-moteur en situation de téléopération : données théoriques et perspectives ergonomiques , 2002 .
[27] G M Redding,et al. Additivity in adaptation to optical tilt. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] L. K. Canon,et al. Influence of concurrent and terminal exposure conditions on the nature of perceptual adaptation. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[29] M. Jeannerod,et al. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[30] C. Prablanc,et al. Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.
[31] B Wallace,et al. Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent exposure: Organization of the eye-hand coordination loop , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[32] C Ghez,et al. Learning of Visuomotor Transformations for Vectorial Planning of Reaching Trajectories , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[33] C. Bard,et al. The Attentional Cost of Amplitude and Directional Requirements When Pointing to Targets , 1994 .
[34] Michel Desmurget,et al. The effect of viewing the static hand prior to movement onset on pointing kinematics and variability , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] James Gordon,et al. Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.
[36] Gordon M. Redding,et al. Visual adaptation to tilt and displacement: Sameor different processes? , 1973 .
[37] G. M. Redding,et al. Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.