Irrelevant sensory stimuli interfere with working memory storage: Evidence from a computational model of prefrontal neurons

The encoding of irrelevant stimuli into the memory store has previously been suggested as a mechanism of interference in working memory (e.g., Lange & Oberauer, Memory, 13, 333–339, 2005; Nairne, Memory & Cognition, 18, 251–269, 1990). Recently, Bancroft and Servos (Experimental Brain Research, 208, 529–532, 2011) used a tactile working memory task to provide experimental evidence that irrelevant stimuli were, in fact, encoded into working memory. In the present study, we replicated Bancroft and Servos’s experimental findings using a biologically based computational model of prefrontal neurons, providing a neurocomputational model of overwriting in working memory. Furthermore, our modeling results show that inhibition acts to protect the contents of working memory, and they suggest a need for further experimental research into the capacity of vibrotactile working memory.

[1]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of a Perceptual Decision in Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[2]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal correlates of parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[3]  Ranulfo Romo,et al.  Neural codes for perceptual discrimination of acoustic flutter in the primate auditory cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Ranulfo Romo,et al.  Flexible Control of Mutual Inhibition: A Neural Model of Two-Interval Discrimination , 2005, Science.

[5]  Tyler D. Bancroft,et al.  Distractor frequency influences performance in vibrotactile working memory , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Supramodal Parametric Working Memory Processing in Humans , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Overwriting of phonemic features in serial recall , 2005, Memory.

[8]  P. Savolainen,et al.  Does navigated TMS of PFC improve tactile working memory by decreasing interference during memory maintenance? , 2008, Brain Stimulation.

[9]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Impairing somatosensory working memory using rTMS , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  E. Rolls,et al.  Decision‐making and Weber's law: a neurophysiological model , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[11]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Interference in verbal working memory: Distinguishing similarity-based confusion, feature overwriting, and feature migration , 2008 .

[12]  Mechanisms of Interference in Vibrotactile Working Memory , 2011, PloS one.

[13]  Facilitation of tactile working memory by top-down suppression from prefrontal to primary somatosensory cortex during sensory interference , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[14]  Ranulfo Romo,et al.  Neural encoding of auditory discrimination in ventral premotor cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  R. Romo,et al.  Temporal Evolution of a Decision-Making Process in Medial Premotor Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[16]  D. McKeown,et al.  Rapid Communication: Interference in Short-Term Auditory Memory , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  R. Engle,et al.  Executive Attention, Working Memory Capacity, and a Two-Factor Theory of Cognitive Control. , 2003 .

[19]  Arno Villringer,et al.  Neural Correlates of Vibrotactile Working Memory in the Human Brain , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  J. S. Nairne A feature model of immediate memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[21]  Marc-Oliver Gewaltig,et al.  Towards Reproducible Descriptions of Neuronal Network Models , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[22]  R. Romo,et al.  Neural codes for perceptual discrimination in primary somatosensory cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Romo,et al.  Timing and neural encoding of somatosensory parametric working memory in macaque prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal correlates of decision-making in secondary somatosensory cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Can vibrotactile working memory store multiple items? , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[26]  Fred Tam,et al.  Functional MRI of working memory and selective attention in vibrotactile frequency discrimination , 2007, BMC Neuroscience.

[27]  C. Eliasmith,et al.  Higher-Dimensional Neurons Explain the Tuning and Dynamics of Working Memory Cells , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Romo,et al.  A recurrent network model of somatosensory parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  Petri Savolainen,et al.  Increasing top-down suppression from prefrontal cortex facilitates tactile working memory , 2010, NeuroImage.

[30]  William E. Hockley,et al.  Vibrotactile Working Memory as a Model Paradigm for Psychology, Neuroscience, and Computational Modeling , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[31]  R. Engle Working Memory Capacity as Executive Attention , 2002 .

[32]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  Working Memory Capacity , 2005 .

[33]  I. Neath Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[34]  Tom Verguts How to Compare Two Quantities? A Computational Model of Flutter Discrimination , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Paul Miller,et al.  Heterogenous Population Coding of a Short-Term Memory and Decision Task , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Interference between storage and processing in working memory: Feature overwriting, not similarity-based competition , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[37]  Paul Miller,et al.  Inhibitory control by an integral feedback signal in prefrontal cortex: a model of discrimination between sequential stimuli. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  A formal model of capacity limits in working memory , 2006 .

[39]  Saskia Haegens,et al.  Somatosensory Anticipatory Alpha Activity Increases to Suppress Distracting Input , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  The Topography of Tactile Learning in Humans , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Emilio Salinas,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Flutter Discrimination: neural codes, perception, memory and decision making , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[42]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Stimulus-dependent EEG activity reflects internal updating of tactile working memory in humans , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  Felix Blankenburg,et al.  Oscillatory Correlates of Vibrotactile Frequency Processing in Human Working Memory , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.