The Framing Effect with Rectangular and Trapezoidal Surfaces: Actual and Pictorial Surface Slant, Frame Orientation, and Viewing Condition
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] W. Hell,et al. Movement parallax: An asymptotic function of amplitude and velocity of head motion , 1978, Vision Research.
[3] B. Rogers,et al. Simultaneous and Successive Contrast Effects in the Perception of Depth from Motion-Parallax and Stereoscopic Information , 1982, Perception.
[4] S. Shimojo,et al. Motion capture changes to induced motion at higher luminance contrasts, smaller eccentricities, and larger inducer sizes , 1993, Vision Research.
[5] R. B. Freeman,et al. Detectability of motion as a factor in depth perception by monocular movement parallax , 1977 .
[6] Herbert F. Crovitz,et al. Perceived length and the Craik-O'Brien illusion , 1976, Vision Research.
[7] A H Reinhardt-Rutland,et al. Perceiving Surface Orientation: Pictorial Information Based on Rectangularity Can Be Overriden during Observer Motion , 1993, Perception.
[8] W. Gehringer,et al. Effect of ecological viewing conditions on the Ames' distorted room illusion. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] H. A. Sedgwick,et al. The effects of viewpoint on the virtual space of pictures , 1991 .
[10] W EPSTEIN,et al. Apparent Shape of a Meaningful Representational Form , 1962, Perceptual and Motor Skills.
[11] E. Eriksson. MONOCULAR SLANT PERCEPTION AND THE TEXTURE GRADIENT CONCEPT , 1964 .
[12] A H Reinhardt-Rutland,et al. Perceiving the Orientation in Depth of Real Surfaces: Background Pattern Affects Motion and Pictorial Information , 1995, Perception.
[13] W. Clark,et al. Retinal gradients of outline distortion and binocular disparity as stimuli for slant. , 1956, Canadian journal of psychology.
[14] D W Eby,et al. The Perceptual Flattening of Three-Dimensional Scenes Enclosed by a Frame , 1995, Perception.
[15] A H Reinhardt-Rutland,et al. Detecting orientation of a surface: the rectangularity postulate and primary depth cues. , 1990, The Journal of general psychology.
[16] A. Reinhardt-Rutland,et al. Verbal Judgments of a Surface's Orientation-In-Depth in Degrees of Angle: Equidistance Tendency, Motion Ineffectiveness, and Automaticity , 1995 .
[17] W. Clark,et al. The interaction of surface texture, outline gradient, and ground in the perception of slant. , 1956, Canadian journal of psychology.
[18] A. Reinhardt-Rutland,et al. Perceiving the orientation-in-depth of triangular surfaces: static-monocular, moving-monocular, and static-binocular viewing. , 1996, The Journal of general psychology.
[19] Allen Brookes,et al. Integrating stereopsis with monocular interpretations of planar surfaces , 1988, Vision Research.
[20] W. Gogel,et al. EQUIDISTANCE TENDENCY AND ITS CONSEQUENCES. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.
[21] John M Foley,et al. Effect of Distance Information and Range on Two Indices of Visually Perceived Distance , 1977, Perception.
[22] W. Gogel,et al. THE TENDENCY TO SEE OBJECTS AS EQUIDISTANT AND ITS INVERSE RELATION TO LATERAL SEPARATION , 1956 .
[23] Stanley N. Roscoe,et al. Bigness Is in the Eye of the Beholder , 1985, Human factors.
[24] W C Gogel,et al. The sensing of retinal size. , 1969, Vision research.
[25] N J Wade,et al. On Interocular Transfer of the Movement Aftereffect in Individuals with and without Normal Binocular Vision , 1976, Perception.