Differential effects of maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress on social-emotional functioning.

Associations between maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress and infant social-emotional adjustment were examined in a subset of dyads from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care (N = 376). Mothers reported on infant temperament at 1 and 6 months postpartum, and maternal sensitivity to distress and nondistress were observed at 6 months. Child behavior problems, social competence, and affect dysregulation were measured at 24 and 36 months. Maternal sensitivity to distress but not to nondistress was related to fewer behavioral problems and higher social competence. In addition, for temperamentally reactive infants, maternal sensitivity to distress was associated with less affect dysregulation. Sensitivity to nondistress only prevented affect dysregulation if sensitivity to distress was also high.

[1]  D. Stern The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology , 2019 .

[2]  E. Leerkes,et al.  Predicting aggressive behavior in the third year from infant reactivity and regulation as moderated by maternal behavior , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.

[3]  J. Belsky,et al.  For Better and For Worse , 2007 .

[4]  R. Bradley,et al.  Infant temperament, parenting, and externalizing behavior in first grade: a test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis. , 2007, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[5]  Laudan B. Jahromi,et al.  Individual Differences in the Contribution of Maternal Soothing to Infant Distress Reduction. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[6]  R. Cattell,et al.  The Procrustes Program: Producing direct rotation to test a hypothesized factor structure. , 2007 .

[7]  Daniel J. Bauer,et al.  Computational Tools for Probing Interactions in Multiple Linear Regression, Multilevel Modeling, and Latent Curve Analysis , 2006 .

[8]  D. Klein,et al.  Positive emotionality at age 3 predicts cognitive styles in 7-year-old children , 2006, Development and Psychopathology.

[9]  Nancy L. McElwain,et al.  Maternal sensitivity to infant distress and nondistress as predictors of infant-mother attachment security. , 2006, Journal of family psychology : JFP : journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association.

[10]  M. Shiota Silver linings and candles in the dark: differences among positive coping strategies in predicting subjective well-being. , 2006, Emotion.

[11]  E. Leerkes,et al.  Infant and maternal behavior moderate reactivity to novelty to predict anxious behavior at 2.5 years , 2006, Development and Psychopathology.

[12]  J. Grusec,et al.  Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. , 2006, Child development.

[13]  R. Fivush,et al.  Emotion Talk in Mother-Child Conversations of the Shared Past: The Effects of Culture, Gender, and Event Valence , 2005 .

[14]  A. Acock Working With Missing Values , 2005 .

[15]  G. Kochanska,et al.  Temperament, relationships, and young children's receptive cooperation with their parents. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[16]  Celia A. Brownell,et al.  Affect dysregulation in the mother-child relationship in the toddler years: antecedents and consequences. , 2004, Development and psychopathology.

[17]  E. Leerkes,et al.  Infant and maternal behaviors regulate infant reactivity to novelty at 6 months. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[18]  S. Calkins,et al.  Infants' vagal regulation in the still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of mother-infant interaction. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[19]  B. Fredrickson,et al.  Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  Mothers’ Regulation Strategies in Response to Toddlers’ Affect: Links to Later Emotion Self-Regulation , 2004 .

[21]  Lisa J. Berlin,et al.  Mothers’ Self‐Reported Control of Their Preschool Children's Emotional Expressiveness: A Longitudinal Study of Associations with Infant–Mother Attachment and Children's Emotion Regulation , 2003 .

[22]  E. DeMulder,et al.  Preschool emotional competence: pathway to social competence? , 2003, Child development.

[23]  Richard A. Fabes,et al.  The Coping with Children's Negative Emotions scale (CCNES): Psychometric properties and relations with Children's emotional competence , 2002 .

[24]  Richard A. Fabes,et al.  Richard A. Fabes, Department of Family & Human Development, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-2502 (E-mail , 2002 .

[25]  S. Calkins,et al.  Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional, behavioral and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. , 2001 .

[26]  G. Bohlin,et al.  Attachment and social functioning: A longitudinal study from infancy to middle childhood , 2000 .

[27]  J. Jenkins,et al.  Confidence in Protection: Arguments for a Narrow Definition of Attachment , 1999 .

[28]  J. Belsky,et al.  Child Care and Mother-Child Interaction in the First Three Years of Life , 1999 .

[29]  M. Reiser,et al.  Parental reactions to children's negative emotions: longitudinal relations to quality of children's social functioning. , 1999, Child development.

[30]  J. Schafer Multiple imputation: a primer , 1999, Statistical methods in medical research.

[31]  Bonnie Knoke,et al.  Early Child Care and Self-Control, Compliance and Problem Behavior at Twenty-four and Thirty-six Months of Age , 1998 .

[32]  J. Belsky,et al.  Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys' externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? , 1998, Development and Psychopathology.

[33]  Ross A. Thompson,et al.  Sensitivity and Security: New Questions to Ponder , 1997 .

[34]  Jay Belsky,et al.  Variation in Susceptibility to Environmental Influence: An Evolutionary Argument , 1997 .

[35]  J. Hyde,et al.  Length of maternity leave and quality of mother-infant interactions. , 1997, Child development.

[36]  J. Belsky,et al.  Infant emotionality, parenting, and 3-year inhibition: exploring stability and lawful discontinuity in a male sample. , 1997, Developmental psychology.

[37]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Responsive Parenting in the Second Year: Specific Influences on Children's Language and Play , 1996 .

[38]  John M. Gottman,et al.  Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. , 1996 .

[39]  D. C. Boom,et al.  Do First-Year Intervention Effects Endure? Follow-Up during Toddlerhood of a Sample of Dutch Irritable Infants. , 1995 .

[40]  G. Kochanska Children's Temperament, Mothers' Discipline, and Security of Attachment: Multiple Pathways to Emerging Internalization , 1995 .

[41]  D. Pederson,et al.  A Categorical Description of Infant-mother Relationships in the Home and its Relation to Q-sort Measures of Infant-mother Interaction , 1995 .

[42]  D. C. Boom,et al.  The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: an experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. , 1994 .

[43]  Ross A. Thompson Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[44]  J. Cassidy Emotion regulation: influences of attachment relationships. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[45]  S. Calkins Origins and outcomes of individual differences in emotion regulation. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[46]  B. Medoff‐Cooper,et al.  The Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire , 1993, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[47]  D. Matsumoto Ethnic differences in affect intensity, emotion judgments, display rule attitudes, and self-reported emotional expression in an American sample , 1993 .

[48]  F. A. Pedersen,et al.  Dyadic distress management predicts subsequent security of attachment , 1993 .

[49]  C. Bauer,et al.  The Adaptive Social Behavior Inventory (Asbi): A New Assessment of Social Competence in High-Risk Three-Year-Olds , 1992 .

[50]  Kevin MacDonald,et al.  Warmth as a Developmental Construct: An Evolutionary Analysis , 1992 .

[51]  T. Achenbach Manual for the child behavior checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile , 1991 .

[52]  D. Rubin Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys , 1989 .

[53]  S C McDevitt,et al.  Revision of the Infant Temperament Questionnaire. , 1978, Pediatrics.

[54]  S. Chess,et al.  Temperament and Development , 1977 .

[55]  L. Radloff The CES-D Scale , 1977 .

[56]  M. Ainsworth,et al.  Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. , 1972, Child development.

[57]  J. Bowlby Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. , 1969, The American journal of orthopsychiatry.

[58]  Seymour Geisser,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1963 .

[59]  H. Kaiser The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis , 1958 .

[60]  S. Calkins,et al.  Temperament in early development. , 2006 .

[61]  Joseph L Schafer,et al.  Multiple Imputation for incomplete multivariate data under a latent-class selection model , 2006 .

[62]  Tracy L. Spinrad,et al.  Parental Socialization of Emotion. , 1998, Psychological inquiry.

[63]  S. Calkins,et al.  Toddler regulation of distress to frustrating events: Temperamental and maternal correlates. , 1998 .

[64]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Mothers' Reactions to Children's Negative Emotions: Relations to Children's Temperament and Anger Behavior. , 1994 .

[65]  S. West,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. , 1994 .

[66]  D. Stern The interpersonal world of infant , 1985 .

[67]  Ross Buck,et al.  The communication of emotion , 1984 .

[68]  A Handbook of Child Psychology , 1932, Nature.