Learning in tactile channels.

Vibrotactile intensity-discrimination thresholds for sinusoidal stimuli applied to the thenar eminence of the hand declined as a function of practice. However, improvement was confined to the tactile information-processing channel in which learning had occurred. Specifically, improvements in performance with training within the Pacinian-corpuscle (PC) channel with a 250-Hz stimulus failed to transfer to performance within the rapidly adapting (RA) nerve fiber channel and the slowly adapting Type I (SA I) nerve fiber channel with a 20-Hz stimulus; similarly, improvements in performance with training within the RA/SA I channels failed to transfer to the PC channel. The hypothesis that tactile intensity-discrimination learning involves changes in sensory processes rather than the acquisition of the general skills required to perform the intensity-discrimination task is supported by the finding that improvements in performance with practice did not transfer to the untrained contralateral hand and by the finding of no transfer of training across channels. Within the PC channel, improvements in intensity discrimination with training transferred from the training intensity level (20-dB SL) to an untrained intensity level (10-dB SL), showing that learning within the PC channel under a specific stimulus condition can generalize to another stimulus condition provided both stimuli are processed by the PC channel. The finding that intensity-discrimination training, which resulted in a substantial reduction in the intensity difference limen, had no effect on the slope of the sensation-magnitude function suggests that tactile intensity-discrimination learning results primarily from a progressive reduction in neural noise over the course of training.

[1]  John H. Wright,et al.  Information-Processing Channels in the Tactile Sensory System: A Psychophysical and Physiological Analysis , 2008 .

[2]  J. Rothwell Principles of Neural Science , 1982 .

[3]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Topographic reorganization of the hand representation in cortical area 3b owl monkeys trained in a frequency-discrimination task. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  K. Sathian,et al.  Perceptual learning in tactile hyperacuity: complete intermanual transfer but limited retention , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  G A Gescheider,et al.  Effect of prior stimulation on vibrotactile thresholds. , 1977, Sensory processes.

[6]  J J Zwislocki,et al.  On the relations of intensity jnd's to loudness and neural noise. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[7]  R T Verrillo,et al.  Vibrotacile masking of Pacinian and non-Pacinian channels. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Progressive improvement in discriminative abilities in adult owl monkeys performing a tactile frequency discrimination task. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  B. Whitsel,et al.  Duration-dependent response of SI to vibrotactile stimulation in squirrel monkey. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Changes in the distributed temporal response properties of SI cortical neurons reflect improvements in performance on a temporally based tactile discrimination task. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Roland S. Johansson,et al.  Tactile Afferent Units with Small and Well Demarcated Receptive Fields in the Glabrous Skin Area of the Human Hand , 1979 .

[12]  D. O. Hebb,et al.  The organization of behavior , 1988 .

[13]  J. F. Dammann,et al.  The Neural Coding of Stimulus Intensity: Linking the Population Response of Mechanoreceptive Afferents with Psychophysical Behavior , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Four channels mediate the mechanical aspects of touch. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  D. Kenshalo Sensory Functions of the Skin of Humans , 2011, Springer US.

[16]  Jozef J. Zwislocki Sensory Neuroscience: Four Laws of Psychophysics , 2008 .

[17]  George A. Gescheider,et al.  Enhancement and summation in the perception of two successive vibrotactile stimuli , 1975 .

[18]  A A Collins,et al.  The measurement of loudness in individual children and adults by absolute magnitude estimation and cross-modality matching. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  G A Gescheider,et al.  Vibrotactile forward masking: evidence for channel independence. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  S J Bolanowski,et al.  Temperature and criterion effects in a somatosensory subsystem: a neurophysiological and psychophysical study. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  James C Craig,et al.  Convergence of submodality-specific input onto neurons in primary somatosensory cortex. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  S J Bolanowski,et al.  The effects of masking on the growth of vibrotactile sensation magnitude and on the amplitude difference limen: a test of the equal sensation magnitude-equal difference limen hypothesis. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[23]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  The effects of skin temperature on the psychophysical responses to vibration on glabrous and hairy skin. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  G A Gescheider,et al.  Prediction of vibrotactile masking functions. , 1982, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  Li Min Chen,et al.  Modality maps within primate somatosensory cortex. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  J J Zwislocki,et al.  On a psychophysical transformed-rule up and down method converging on a 75% level of correct responses , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  G A Gescheider,et al.  Vibrotactile temporal summation for threshold and suprathreshold levels of stimulation , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Sliman J. Bensmaia,et al.  Tactile intensity and population codes , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Society for Neuroscience Abstracts , 1997 .

[30]  James C. Craig,et al.  Vibrotactile difference thresholds for intensity and the effect of a masking stimulus , 1974 .

[31]  W M Jenkins,et al.  Frequency discrimination training engaging a restricted skin surface results in an emergence of a cutaneous response zone in cortical area 3a. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  M. Teghtsoonian,et al.  On the relation between the growth of loudness and the discrimination of intensity for pure tones. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  G A Gescheider,et al.  Psychophysical tuning curves in vibrotaction. , 1978, Sensory processes.

[34]  G A Gescheider,et al.  Vibrotactile forward masking: psychophysical evidence for a triplex theory of cutaneous mechanoreception. , 1985, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  S J Bolanowski,et al.  Vibrotactile temporal summation: probability summation or neural integration? , 1999, Somatosensory & motor research.

[36]  R T Verrillo,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for a triplex system of cutaneous mechanoreception. , 1979, Sensory processes.

[37]  G. Gescheider Psychophysics: The Fundamentals , 1997 .

[38]  M. Hollins,et al.  Vibrotactile adaptation enhances amplitude discrimination. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[39]  M. Hollins,et al.  Perceived intensity of vibrotactile stimuli: the role of mechanoreceptive channels. , 1996, Somatosensory & motor research.

[40]  K. Sathian,et al.  Tactile learning is task specific but transfers between fingers , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  The frequency selectivity of information-processing channels in the tactile sensory system , 2001, Somatosensory & motor research.

[42]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  The Topography of Tactile Learning in Humans , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  R H Margolis,et al.  Just noticeable differences for intensity and their relation to loudness. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[44]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Vibrotactile intensity discrimination measured by three methods. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[45]  Jason C. Cohen,et al.  Under which conditions do the skin and probe decouple during sinusoidal vibrations? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  D G Pelli,et al.  Effects of noise on detection of amplitude increments of sinusoidal vibration of the skin. , 1992, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[47]  L E Marks,et al.  Summation of vibrotactile intensity: an analog to auditory critical bands? , 1979, Sensory processes.

[48]  J A Stillman,et al.  Intensity just-noticeable differences at equal-loudness levels in normal and pathological ears. , 1993, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[49]  S. Bolanowski,et al.  Perception of the tactile texture of raised-dot patterns: A multidimensional analysis , 2005, Somatosensory & motor research.

[50]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[51]  G A Gescheider,et al.  Selective adaptation of vibrotactile thresholds. , 1979, Sensory processes.