Toward a formal theory of flexible spatial behavior: geometric category biases generalize across pointing and verbal response types.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Plumert,et al. The development of memory for location: what role do spatial prototypes play? , 2001, Child development.
[2] A. Georgopoulos,et al. Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex. , 1993, Science.
[3] John P Spencer,et al. Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[4] H L Pick,et al. Children's use of frames of reference in communication of spatial location. , 1990, Child development.
[5] Leonard Talmy,et al. How Language Structures Space , 1983 .
[6] Gregor Schöner,et al. The Emergence of Stimulus-Response Associations from Neural Activation Fields: Dynamic Field Theory , 2005 .
[7] Diane J. Schiano,et al. Perceptual and conceptual factors in distortions in memory for graphs and maps. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[8] M. Fahle,et al. Grasping Visual Illusions: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action , 2000, Psychological science.
[9] R. Mansfield,et al. Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .
[10] Anne R. Schutte,et al. Testing the dynamic field theory: working memory for locations becomes more spatially precise over development. , 2003, Child development.
[11] J. Huttenlocher,et al. The development of hierarchical representation of two-dimensional space. , 1996, Child development.
[12] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .
[13] J. Plumert,et al. The Early Development of Children's Communication about Nested Spatial Relations , 1995 .
[14] B McCabe,et al. Memory for locations within regions: Spatial biases and visual hemifield differences , 1998, Memory & cognition.
[15] J. Huttenlocher,et al. The Coding of Spatial Location in Young Children , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[17] Jörn Diedrichsen,et al. The time course of spatial memory distortions , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[18] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.
[19] Anne R. Schutte,et al. Unifying Representations and Responses , 2004, Psychological science.
[20] J. Mandler,et al. Early Sensitivity to Language-Specific Spatial Categories in English and Korean , 1999 .
[21] Linda B. Smith,et al. Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. , 1999, Psychological review.
[22] B. Bridgeman. Separate visual representations for perception and for visually guided behavior , 1991 .
[23] John P. Spencer,et al. An Embodied Approach to Cognitive Systems: A Dynamic Neural Field Theory of Spatial Working Memory , 2006 .
[24] Robert Cohen,et al. The Development of spatial cognition , 1985 .
[25] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Visuomotor Transformations for Reaching to Memorized Targets: A PET Study , 1997, NeuroImage.
[26] Linda B. Smith,et al. Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: the influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of two-year-olds. , 2001, Child development.
[27] Bruce Bridgeman,et al. Processing spatial information in the sensorimotor branch of the visual system , 2000, Vision Research.
[28] Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al. Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.
[29] J E Cutting,et al. Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] T. O. Nelson,et al. Immediate memory for spatial location. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[31] M. Goodale,et al. Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.
[32] J. F. Soechting,et al. Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[33] J. Plumert,et al. Parental Scaffolding of Young Children's Spatial Communication , 1996 .
[34] Anne R. Schutte,et al. Generalizing the dynamic field theory of the A-not-B error beyond infancy: three-year-olds' delay- and experience-dependent location memory biases. , 2002, Child development.
[35] M. Fahle,et al. Grasp effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion: obstacle avoidance is not the explanation , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.
[36] E. Brenner,et al. Independent movements of the digits in grasping , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] E. Spelke,et al. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.
[38] B. Bridgeman,et al. Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. The Effect of Pictorial Illusion on Prehension and Perception , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] N I Durlach,et al. Evaluation of response methods for the localization of nearby objects , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[41] T. Bachmann,et al. Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (Advances in Psychology, Vol. 129) , 1999 .
[42] L. Hedges,et al. Categories and particulars: prototype effects in estimating spatial location. , 1991, Psychological review.
[43] P. Bloom,et al. Language and space , 1998 .
[44] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[45] Nora S. Newcombe,et al. Infants' coding of location in continuous space. , 1999 .
[46] F. Lacquaniti,et al. Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[47] J. Huttenlocher,et al. Bias in spatial location due to categorization: comment on Tversky and Schiano. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[48] B. Fischhoff,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .
[49] Gregor Schöner,et al. Reference-related inhibition produces enhanced position discrimination and fast repulsion near axes of symmetry , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] L. Hermer-Vazquez,et al. Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks , 2001, Cognition.
[51] B. Bridgeman,et al. Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[52] T. Regier,et al. Linguistic and non-linguistic spatial categorization , 2000, Cognition.
[53] Gordon D. Logan,et al. A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations , 1996 .
[54] John P Spencer,et al. Prototypes and particulars: geometric and experience-dependent spatial categories. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[55] B. Pasamanick. The Construction of Reality in the Child , 1955 .
[56] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. Robust Spatial Working Memory through Homeostatic Synaptic Scaling in Heterogeneous Cortical Networks , 2003, Neuron.
[57] Laura A. Carlson-Radvansky,et al. The Influence of Reference Frame Selection on Spatial Template Construction , 1997 .
[58] Susan J. Hespos,et al. Conceptual precursors to language , 2004, Nature.
[59] P. Goldman-Rakic. Cellular basis of working memory , 1995, Neuron.
[60] G. Schöner,et al. Bridging the representational gap in the dynamic systems approach to development , 2003 .
[61] Alexandre Pouget,et al. A computational perspective on the neural basis of multisensory spatial representations , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[62] Jack L. Vevea,et al. Why do categories affect stimulus judgment? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[63] Estela Bicho,et al. Target Representation on an Autonomous Vehicle with Low-Level Sensors , 2000, Int. J. Robotics Res..
[64] Bruce Bridgeman,et al. Chapter 1 Separate representations of visual space for perception and visually guided behavior , 1999 .
[65] Peter M. Vishton,et al. Effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion on different behaviors: one- and two-handed grasping; one- and two-handed manual estimation; metric and comparative judgment. , 2003, Spatial vision.
[66] R. Jackendoff. The architecture of the linguistic-spatial interface , 1996 .
[67] E. Thelen,et al. The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[68] P. Goldman-Rakic,et al. Synaptic mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working memory in a cortical network model. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[69] Jodie M Plumert,et al. Delay-induced bias in children's memory for location. , 2002, Child development.
[70] Diane J. Schiano,et al. Structure and strategy in encoding simplified graphs , 1992, Memory & cognition.
[71] Rich Gossweiler,et al. Perceiving geographical slant , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.