Is Region-of-Interest Overlap Comparison a Reliable Measure of Category Specificity?

Analysis of the degree of overlap between functional magnetic resonance imagingderived regions of interest (ROIs) has been used to assess the functional convergence and/or segregation of category-selective brain areas. An examination of the extant literature reveals no consistent usage for how such overlap is calculated, nor any systematic comparison between different methods. We argue that how ROI overlap is computed, especially the choice of the denominator in the formula, can profoundly affect the results and interpretation of such an analysis. To do this, we compared the overlap of the FFA-FFA (fusiform face area) and FFA-FGA (fusiform Greeble-selective area) in a localizer study testing both Greeble novices and experts. When using a single ROI as the denominator, we found a significant difference in FFA-FFA versus FFA-FGA overlap, consistent with the result of a previous study arguing for face specificity of the FFA [Rhodes, G., Byatt, G., Michie, P. T., & Puce, A. Is the fusiform face area specialized for faces, individuation, or expert individuation? J Cogn Neurosci, 16, 189203, 2004]. However, these ROI overlap differences disappeared when the denominator combined both of the involved ROIs, and the patterns of such overlap comparisons were dependent on given statistical thresholds. We also found proportionally decreasing FFA-FFA overlap with increasing center-of-FFA distance, resolving an apparent contradiction between the consistency of the location of the FFA and the seemingly low FFA-FFA overlap. Finally, Monte Carlo simulations revealed the most stable formulathe most resistant to ROI size variationsto be the average of the two single-ROI-denominator-based overlap indices. In sum, ROI overlap analysis is not a reliable tool for assessing category specificity, and caution should be exercised with regard to ROI overlap definition, underlying assumptions, and interpretation.

[1]  Satrajit S. Ghosh,et al.  Region of interest based analysis of functional imaging data , 2003, NeuroImage.

[2]  Nancy Kanwisher Neuroscience. What's in a face? , 2006, Science.

[3]  Michael Erb,et al.  Object-selective responses in the human motion area MT/MST , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Domain specificity in visual cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Expert Skills in Visual Working Memory , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Is the Fusiform Face Area Specialized for Faces, Individuation, or Expert Individuation? , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  N. Kanwisher Domain specificity in face perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Alice J. O'Toole,et al.  Partially Distributed Representations of Objects and Faces in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  M. Tarr,et al.  Activation of the middle fusiform 'face area' increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Mental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific Brain Regions , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Rebecca F. Schwarzlose,et al.  Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Cindy M. Bukach,et al.  Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[16]  Thomas Dierks,et al.  Dissociation between overt and unconscious face processing in fusiform face area , 2004, NeuroImage.

[17]  Svetlana S. Georgieva,et al.  Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging to Assess Adaptation and Size Invariance of Shape Processing by Humans and Monkeys , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.

[19]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Measuring selectivity in fMRI data , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Tarr,et al.  Levels of categorization in visual recognition studied using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Current Biology.

[21]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Dissecting Contributions of Prefrontal Cortex and Fusiform Face Area to Face Working Memory , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  M. Tarr,et al.  Becoming a “Greeble” Expert: Exploring Mechanisms for Face Recognition , 1997, Vision Research.

[23]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  T. Allison,et al.  Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cognitive Conjunction: A New Approach to Brain Activation Experiments , 1997, NeuroImage.

[26]  J. Tanaka The entry point of face recognition: evidence for face expertise. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Is the extrastriate body area involved in motor actions? , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Tanaka,et al.  Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  R. Malach,et al.  Intersubject Synchronization of Cortical Activity During Natural Vision , 2004, Science.

[31]  P. Downing,et al.  Within‐subject reproducibility of category‐specific visual activation with functional MRI , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[32]  Cathy J. Price,et al.  Fusiform Activation to Animals is Driven by the Process, Not the Stimulus , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  K. Nakayama,et al.  RESPONSE PROPERTIES OF THE HUMAN FUSIFORM FACE AREA , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Multisubject fMRI Studies and Conjunction Analyses , 1999, NeuroImage.

[35]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[36]  Katie L. McMahon,et al.  Action word meaning representations in cytoarchitectonically defined primary and premotor cortices , 2008, NeuroImage.

[37]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Does the human brain process objects of expertise like faces? A review of the evidence , 2005 .

[38]  M. Tarr,et al.  Training ‘greeble’ experts: a framework for studying expert object recognition processes , 1998, Vision Research.

[39]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Does the fusiform face area contain subregions highly selective for nonfaces? , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  D. Heeger,et al.  Linear Systems Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Human V1 , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  R. Kiani,et al.  Microstimulation of inferotemporal cortex influences face categorization , 2006, Nature.

[42]  P. Downing,et al.  Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  What's in a Face? , 2006, Science.

[44]  M. Denis,et al.  Reopening the Mental Imagery Debate: Lessons from Functional Anatomy , 1998, NeuroImage.

[45]  M. Tarr,et al.  Visual expertise with nonface objects leads to competition with the early perceptual processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  M. Chee,et al.  Overlap and Dissociation of Semantic Processing of Chinese Characters, English Words, and Pictures: Evidence from fMRI , 2000, NeuroImage.

[47]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Discrimination Training Alters Object Representations in Human Extrastriate Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  M. Tarr,et al.  FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Neuroanatomic Overlap of Working Memory and Spatial Attention Networks: A Functional MRI Comparison within Subjects , 1999, NeuroImage.

[50]  Elinor McKone,et al.  The evidence rejects the expertise hypothesis: Reply to Gauthier & Bukach , 2007, Cognition.

[51]  Jane E Joseph,et al.  Natural and manufactured objects activate the fusiform face area , 2002, Neuroreport.

[52]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural foundations of imagery , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[53]  Stephen José Hanson,et al.  Combinatorial codes in ventral temporal lobe for object recognition: Haxby (2001) revisited: is there a “face” area? , 2004, NeuroImage.

[54]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Should we reject the expertise hypothesis? , 2007, Cognition.

[55]  M. Tarr,et al.  DOES VISUAL SUBORDINATE-LEVEL CATEGORISATION ENGAGE THE FUNCTIONALLY DEFINED FUSIFORM FACE AREA? , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[56]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .

[58]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Conjunction revisited , 2005, NeuroImage.

[59]  David Caplan,et al.  Cognitive conjunction and cognitive functions , 2004, NeuroImage.

[60]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  How Distributed Is Visual Category Information in Human Occipito-Temporal Cortex? An fMRI Study , 2002, Neuron.

[61]  K. Nakayama,et al.  The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area , 1998, Cognition.

[62]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  High-resolution imaging reveals highly selective nonface clusters in the fusiform face area , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.