Selective prefrontal serotonin depletion impairs acquisition of a detour‐reaching task

We have recently shown that serotonin in the primate orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) contributes to the flexible control of behaviour. 5,7‐dihydroxytryptamine‐induced 5‐HT depletions of OFC impair performance on a serial reversal discrimination task [ Clarke et al. (2004)Science, 304, 878–880]. The deficit is characterized by perseverative responding to the previously rewarded stimulus, a deficit similar to that seen following lesions of the intrinsic neurones of the OFC [ Dias et al. (1996)Nature, 380, 69–72]. The effect is neurochemically selective as dopaminergic lesions of the OFC, induced by 6‐hydroxydopamine, have no effect [ Clarke et al. (2006)Cerebral Cortex]. In order to test for the generality of the effect of serotonin on orbitofrontal processing and, in particular, its effects on flexible behaviour, the present study investigated the effects of serotonin depletions of OFC on performance of another task dependent upon an intact OFC, the detour‐reaching task [ Wallis et al. (2001)European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1797–1808]. Successful performance of this task requires inhibition of the animal's prepotent response tendency to reach directly along its line of sight to the reward. Compared with sham‐operated controls, we found that lesioned monkeys made significantly more barrier reaches directly along their line of sight to the visible reward during task acquisition. This finding provides further support for the role of prefrontal serotonin in inhibitory control processes specifically in tasks sensitive to OFC dysfunction.

[1]  J. Zohar,et al.  Paroxetine versus Clomipramine in the Treatment of Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder , 1996, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[2]  E. Walderhaug,et al.  Lowering of serotonin by rapid tryptophan depletion increases impulsiveness in normal individuals , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[3]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion is behaviorally and neurochemically specific. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Trevor W Robbins,et al.  Fractionating Impulsivity: Contrasting Effects of Central 5-HT Depletion on Different Measures of Impulsive Behavior , 2004, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[5]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and lateral prefrontal cortex of the marmoset to performance on a detour reaching task , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  Dawn M Eagle,et al.  Deficits in Impulse Control Associated with Tonically-elevated Serotonergic Function in Rat Prefrontal Cortex , 2002, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[7]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[8]  T. Robbins,et al.  Doubly dissociable effects of median- and dorsal-raphé lesions on the performance of the five-choice serial reaction time test of attention in rats , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[9]  E. Rolls,et al.  Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[10]  J. Evenden Varieties of impulsivity , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[11]  S. Mobini,et al.  Effects of central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[12]  Z. Šidák Rectangular Confidence Regions for the Means of Multivariate Normal Distributions , 1967 .

[13]  Barry J. Everitt,et al.  Central 5-HT depletion enhances impulsive responding without affecting the accuracy of attentional performance: interactions with dopaminergic mechanisms , 1997, Psychopharmacology.

[14]  J. P. Morgan,et al.  Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 2005, Technometrics.

[15]  S. Klein,et al.  Handbook of contemporary learning theories , 2000 .

[16]  E. Hollander Treatment of obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders with SSRIs , 1998, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[17]  Cognitive and motor deficits in the performance of an object retrieval task with a barrier-detour in monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus) treated with MPTP: long-term performance and effect of transparency of the barrier. , 1990, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Dissociating Valence of Outcome from Behavioral Control in Human Orbital and Ventral Prefrontal Cortices , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Perseveration, inhibition and the prefrontal cortex: a new look , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[20]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive Inflexibility After Prefrontal Serotonin Depletion , 2004, Science.

[21]  Jelliffe Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde , 1910 .

[22]  T. Robbins,et al.  Tryptophan depletion impairs stimulus-reward learning while methylphenidate disrupts attentional control in healthy young adults: implications for the monoaminergic basis of impulsive behaviour , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[23]  R. Roth,et al.  Enduring cognitive deficits and cortical dopamine dysfunction in monkeys after long-term administration of phencyclidine. , 1997, Science.

[24]  K. Brodmann Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde : in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues , 1985 .

[25]  L. Squire,et al.  Successful performance by monkeys with lesions of the hippocampal formation on AB and object retrieval, two tasks that mark developmental changes in human infants. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[26]  T. Robbins,et al.  Prefrontal Serotonin Depletion Affects Reversal Learning But Not Attentional Set Shifting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  S. Lane,et al.  Fenfluramine effects on impulsivity in a sample of adults with and without history of conduct disorder , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[28]  P. Soubrié Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.