Goal Relevance Moderates Evaluative Conditioning Effects.

[1]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  Dissociating contingency awareness and conditioned attitudes: evidence of contingency-unaware evaluative conditioning. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  W. Stroebe,et al.  The workings and limits of subliminal advertising: The role of habits , 2011 .

[3]  C. Unkelbach,et al.  Beyond Evaluative Conditioning! Evidence for Transfer of Non-Evaluative Attributes , 2011 .

[4]  K. Rothermund,et al.  What you see is what will change: Evaluative conditioning effects depend on a focus on valence , 2011, Cognition & emotion.

[5]  Skyler T. Hawk,et al.  Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database , 2010 .

[6]  H. Aarts,et al.  The Unconscious Will: How the Pursuit of Goals Operates Outside of Conscious Awareness , 2010, Science.

[7]  Christopher R. Jones,et al.  Implicit misattribution as a mechanism underlying evaluative conditioning. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  Christoph Stahl,et al.  Evaluative learning with single versus multiple unconditioned stimuli: the role of contingency awareness. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al.  The hidden persuaders break into the tired brain , 2009 .

[10]  J. Houwer The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models , 2009 .

[11]  Maarten W. Bos,et al.  On the goal-dependency of unconscious thought , 2008 .

[12]  W. Stroebe,et al.  Beyond Vicary's fantasies: The impact of subliminal priming and brand choice , 2006 .

[13]  A. Dijksterhuis,et al.  A Theory of Unconscious Thought , 2006, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[14]  F. Baeyens,et al.  Beyond evaluative conditioning? Searching for associative transfer of nonevaluative stimulus properties , 2005, Cognition & emotion.

[15]  P. Pintrich A Conceptual Framework for Assessing Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning in College Students , 2004 .

[16]  John A Bargh,et al.  Liking is for doing: the effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Arie W Kruglanski,et al.  Forgetting all else: on the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  Gordon B. Moskowitz,et al.  Preconscious effects of temporary goals on attention , 2002 .

[19]  H. Aarts,et al.  On the psychology of drinking: being thirsty and perceptually ready. , 2001, British journal of psychology.

[20]  P. Gollwitzer Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .

[21]  G. Crombez,et al.  Observational Conditioning of Food Valence in Humans , 1996, Appetite.

[22]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. , 1994 .

[23]  D. Hermans,et al.  The role of CS-US contingency in human evaluative conditioning. , 1993, Behaviour research and therapy.

[24]  M. J. Houston,et al.  Goal-Oriented Experiences and the Development of Knowledge , 1993 .

[25]  P. Blumenfeld Classroom Learning and Motivation: Clarifying and Expanding Goal Theory , 1992 .

[26]  O. Van den Bergh,et al.  Human evaluative conditioning: acquisition trials, presentation schedule, evaluative style and contingency awareness. , 1992, Behaviour research and therapy.

[27]  J. Garber,et al.  Factors governing the effective remediation of negative affect and its cognitive and behavioral consequences. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  J. Bargh Attention and Automaticity in the Processing of Self-Relevant Information , 1982 .

[29]  M. Perugini,et al.  Evaluative conditioning in humans: a meta-analysis. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  T. Mussweiler,et al.  Beyond awareness and resources: Evaluative conditioning may be sensitive to processing goals , 2009 .

[31]  J. Houwer A Conceptual and Theoretical Analysis of Evaluative Conditioning , 2007 .

[32]  O. Luminet,et al.  Aware and (dis)liking: item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency awareness. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  H. Aarts,et al.  Habits as knowledge structures: automaticity in goal-directed behavior. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  David B. Leake,et al.  Goal-driven learning , 1995 .

[35]  A. Öhman Part VI. Evaluating evaluative conditioning: Some comments on “cognitions, evaluations, and conditioning: Rules of sequence and rules of consequence” by Levey and Martin , 1983 .

[36]  John Garcia,et al.  Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning , 1966 .

[37]  M. Ferguson,et al.  On Becoming Ready to Pursue a Goal You Don't Know You Have: Effects of Nonconscious Goals on Evaluative Readiness How Conscious Goals Lead to Evaluative Readiness Do Nonconscious Goals Lead to Evaluative Readiness? Identifying Moderators of Evaluative Readiness , 2022 .

[38]  O. Corneille,et al.  On the Respective Contributions of Awareness of Unconditioned Stimulus Valence and Unconditioned Stimulus Identity in Attitude Formation through Evaluative Conditioning a Memory-based Criterion of Awareness Examining the Role of Awareness in Ec at the Level of Cs-us Contingencies Contingency Awarene , 2022 .