Distance Estimation Is Influenced by Encoding Conditions

Background It is well established that foveating a behaviorally relevant part of the visual field improves localization performance as compared to the situation where the gaze is directed elsewhere. Reduced localization performance in the peripheral encoding conditions has been attributed to an eccentricity-dependent increase in positional uncertainty. It is not known, however, whether and how the foveal and peripheral encoding conditions can influence spatial interval estimation. In this study we compare observers' estimates of a distance between two co-planar dots in the condition where they foveate the two sample dots and where they fixate a central dot while viewing the sample dots peripherally. Methodology/Principal Findings Observers were required to reproduce, after a short delay, a distance between two sample dots based on a stationary reference dot and a movable mouse pointer. When both sample dots are foveated, we find that the distance estimation error is small but consistently increases with the dots-separation size. In comparison, distance judgment in peripheral encoding condition is significantly overestimated for smaller separations and becomes similar to the performance in foveal trials for distances from 10 to 16 degrees. Conclusions/Significance Although we find improved accuracy of distance estimation in the foveal condition, the fact that the difference is related to the reduction of the estimation bias present in the peripheral conditon, challenges the simple account of reducing the eccentricity-dependent positional uncertainty. Contrary to this, we present evidence for an explanation in terms of neuronal populations activated by the two sample dots and their inhibitory interactions under different visual encoding conditions. We support our claims with simulations that take into account receptive fields size differences between the two encoding conditions.

[1]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.

[2]  C. A. Burbeck Position and spatial frequency in large-scale localization judgments , 1987, Vision Research.

[3]  D. Munoz,et al.  A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  D P Munoz,et al.  Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  “Weber's law” for position: Unconfounding the role of separation and eccentricity , 1988, Vision Research.

[6]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Activation patterns in visual cortex reveal receptive field size-dependent attentional modulation , 2008, Brain Research.

[7]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Reference Repulsion When Judging the Direction of Visual Motion , 1998, Perception.

[8]  D. P. Andrews,et al.  Acuity for spatial separation as a function of stimulus size , 1978, Vision Research.

[9]  H. P. Snippe,et al.  Effects of blur and eccentricity on differential spatial displacement discrimination , 1988, Vision Research.

[10]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Spatial distribution of contextual interactions in primary visual cortex and in visual perception. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  The role of separation and eccentricity in encoding position , 1990, Vision Research.

[12]  Takahiro Kawabe,et al.  Attention shift not memory averaging reduces foveal bias , 2005, Vision Research.

[13]  S. Tremblay,et al.  Rehearsal in serial memory for visual-spatial information: Evidence from eye movements , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[14]  Arash Yazdanbakhsh,et al.  A new psychophysical estimation of the receptive field size , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[15]  Peter Wenderoth,et al.  The tilt illusion: Repulsion and attraction effects in the oblique meridian , 1977, Vision Research.

[16]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Revisiting motion repulsion: evidence for a general phenomenon? , 1999, Vision Research.

[17]  Robert F. Hess,et al.  The coding of spatial position by the human visual system: Effects of spatial scale and retinal eccentricity , 1994, Vision Research.

[18]  Rick Gurnsey,et al.  Non-monotonic changes in performance with eccentricity modeled by multiple eccentricity-dependent limitations , 2005, Vision Research.

[19]  Donald Laming,et al.  Weber's Law , 2008 .

[20]  K Kani,et al.  Retinal sensitivity and spatial summation in the foveal and parafoveal regions. , 1981, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[21]  S. McKee,et al.  Spatial configurations for visual hyperacuity , 1977, Vision Research.

[22]  W. J. Tam,et al.  Fixation disengagement and eye-movement latency , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  M. Carandini,et al.  Predictions of a recurrent model of orientation selectivity , 1997, Vision Research.

[24]  Feng Qi Han,et al.  Excitatory and suppressive receptive field subunits in awake monkey primary visual cortex (V1) , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[27]  G. Westheimer Lines and gabor functions compared as spatial visual stimuli , 1998, Vision Research.

[28]  R. Klein,et al.  What are human express saccades? , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  R. Blake,et al.  Direction repulsion in motion transparency , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[30]  M E Wilson,et al.  Invariant features of spatial summation with changing locus in the visual field , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[31]  H U May,et al.  [Invariances in the cat's retina: principles in the relations between sensitivity, size and position of receptive fields of ganglion cells]. , 1970, Experimental brain research.

[32]  R. Oehler Spatial interactions in the rhesus monkey retina: a behavioural study using the Westheimer paradigm , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Ning Qian,et al.  A physiologically-based explanation of disparity attraction and repulsion , 2000, Vision Research.

[34]  H. U. May,et al.  Invarianzen in der Katzenretina: Gesetzmä\ige beziehungen zwischen Empfindlichkeit, Größe und Lage receptiver Felder von Ganglienzellen , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  R. Johansson,et al.  Engagement of gaze in capturing targets for future sequential manual actions. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  T. Oyama,et al.  Perceptual grouping between successively presented stimuli and its relation to visual simultaneity and masking , 1978, Psychological research.

[37]  R. Vautin,et al.  Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  D. Levi,et al.  Spatial localization without visual references , 1992, Vision Research.

[39]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Compression of space in visual memory , 2001, Vision Research.

[40]  Jean Saint-Aubin,et al.  Eye movements and serial memory for visual-spatial information: does time spent fixating contribute to recall? , 2007, Experimental psychology.

[41]  R. Klein,et al.  Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: does predisengagement of visuospatial attention mediate this gap effect? , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Peripheral positional acuity: Retinal and cortical constraints on 2-dot separation discrimination under photopic and scotopic conditions , 1989, Vision Research.

[43]  P. Dayan,et al.  Space and time in visual context , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  Lawrence K. Cormack,et al.  Depth attraction and repulsion in random dot stereograms , 1991, Vision Research.

[45]  B. Wandell,et al.  Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Lateral Inhibition between Orientation Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1970, Nature.

[47]  E. L. Schwartz,et al.  Multi-area visuotopic map complexes in macaque striate and extra-striate cortex , 2006, Vision Research.

[48]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[49]  D. Kerzel Memory for the position of stationary objects: disentangling foveal bias and memory averaging , 2002, Vision Research.

[50]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[51]  B. Fischer Overlap of receptive field centers and representation of the visual field in the cat's optic tract. , 1973, Vision research.

[52]  S. Mateeff,et al.  Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.

[53]  H. Bouma,et al.  Induced changes in the perceived orientation of line segments. , 1970, Vision research.

[54]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Maintenance of Spatial and Motor Codes during Oculomotor Delayed Response Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.