Distance Estimation Is Influenced by Encoding Conditions
暂无分享,去创建一个
Albert Postma | Richard J. A. van Wezel | Anna Oleksiak | A. V. van den Berg | Ineke J. M. van der Ham | Albert V. van den Berg | A. Postma | I. V. D. van der Ham | R. van Wezel | A. Oleksiak | Mirosława Mańko | Mirosława Mańko
[1] R. Sekuler,et al. Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.
[2] C. A. Burbeck. Position and spatial frequency in large-scale localization judgments , 1987, Vision Research.
[3] D. Munoz,et al. A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[4] D P Munoz,et al. Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[5] Dennis M. Levi,et al. “Weber's law” for position: Unconfounding the role of separation and eccentricity , 1988, Vision Research.
[6] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Activation patterns in visual cortex reveal receptive field size-dependent attentional modulation , 2008, Brain Research.
[7] Stefan Treue,et al. Reference Repulsion When Judging the Direction of Visual Motion , 1998, Perception.
[8] D. P. Andrews,et al. Acuity for spatial separation as a function of stimulus size , 1978, Vision Research.
[9] H. P. Snippe,et al. Effects of blur and eccentricity on differential spatial displacement discrimination , 1988, Vision Research.
[10] C. Gilbert,et al. Spatial distribution of contextual interactions in primary visual cortex and in visual perception. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] Dennis M. Levi,et al. The role of separation and eccentricity in encoding position , 1990, Vision Research.
[12] Takahiro Kawabe,et al. Attention shift not memory averaging reduces foveal bias , 2005, Vision Research.
[13] S. Tremblay,et al. Rehearsal in serial memory for visual-spatial information: Evidence from eye movements , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[14] Arash Yazdanbakhsh,et al. A new psychophysical estimation of the receptive field size , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.
[15] Peter Wenderoth,et al. The tilt illusion: Repulsion and attraction effects in the oblique meridian , 1977, Vision Research.
[16] Stefan Treue,et al. Revisiting motion repulsion: evidence for a general phenomenon? , 1999, Vision Research.
[17] Robert F. Hess,et al. The coding of spatial position by the human visual system: Effects of spatial scale and retinal eccentricity , 1994, Vision Research.
[18] Rick Gurnsey,et al. Non-monotonic changes in performance with eccentricity modeled by multiple eccentricity-dependent limitations , 2005, Vision Research.
[19] Donald Laming,et al. Weber's Law , 2008 .
[20] K Kani,et al. Retinal sensitivity and spatial summation in the foveal and parafoveal regions. , 1981, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[21] S. McKee,et al. Spatial configurations for visual hyperacuity , 1977, Vision Research.
[22] W. J. Tam,et al. Fixation disengagement and eye-movement latency , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[23] M. Carandini,et al. Predictions of a recurrent model of orientation selectivity , 1997, Vision Research.
[24] Feng Qi Han,et al. Excitatory and suppressive receptive field subunits in awake monkey primary visual cortex (V1) , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[26] Frans W Cornelissen,et al. The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[27] G. Westheimer. Lines and gabor functions compared as spatial visual stimuli , 1998, Vision Research.
[28] R. Klein,et al. What are human express saccades? , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[29] R. Blake,et al. Direction repulsion in motion transparency , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.
[30] M E Wilson,et al. Invariant features of spatial summation with changing locus in the visual field , 1970, The Journal of physiology.
[31] H U May,et al. [Invariances in the cat's retina: principles in the relations between sensitivity, size and position of receptive fields of ganglion cells]. , 1970, Experimental brain research.
[32] R. Oehler. Spatial interactions in the rhesus monkey retina: a behavioural study using the Westheimer paradigm , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[33] Ning Qian,et al. A physiologically-based explanation of disparity attraction and repulsion , 2000, Vision Research.
[34] H. U. May,et al. Invarianzen in der Katzenretina: Gesetzmä\ige beziehungen zwischen Empfindlichkeit, Größe und Lage receptiver Felder von Ganglienzellen , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] R. Johansson,et al. Engagement of gaze in capturing targets for future sequential manual actions. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[36] T. Oyama,et al. Perceptual grouping between successively presented stimuli and its relation to visual simultaneity and masking , 1978, Psychological research.
[37] R. Vautin,et al. Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[38] D. Levi,et al. Spatial localization without visual references , 1992, Vision Research.
[39] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Compression of space in visual memory , 2001, Vision Research.
[40] Jean Saint-Aubin,et al. Eye movements and serial memory for visual-spatial information: does time spent fixating contribute to recall? , 2007, Experimental psychology.
[41] R. Klein,et al. Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: does predisengagement of visuospatial attention mediate this gap effect? , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] Dennis M. Levi,et al. Peripheral positional acuity: Retinal and cortical constraints on 2-dot separation discrimination under photopic and scotopic conditions , 1989, Vision Research.
[43] P. Dayan,et al. Space and time in visual context , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[44] Lawrence K. Cormack,et al. Depth attraction and repulsion in random dot stereograms , 1991, Vision Research.
[45] B. Wandell,et al. Visual field maps, population receptive field sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MT+ complex. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[46] C. Blakemore,et al. Lateral Inhibition between Orientation Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1970, Nature.
[47] E. L. Schwartz,et al. Multi-area visuotopic map complexes in macaque striate and extra-striate cortex , 2006, Vision Research.
[48] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[49] D. Kerzel. Memory for the position of stationary objects: disentangling foveal bias and memory averaging , 2002, Vision Research.
[50] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[51] B. Fischer. Overlap of receptive field centers and representation of the visual field in the cat's optic tract. , 1973, Vision research.
[52] S. Mateeff,et al. Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.
[53] H. Bouma,et al. Induced changes in the perceived orientation of line segments. , 1970, Vision research.
[54] Clayton E. Curtis,et al. Maintenance of Spatial and Motor Codes during Oculomotor Delayed Response Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.