Tactile suppression of displacement

In vision, the discovery of the phenomenon of saccadic suppression of displacement has made important contributions to the understanding of the stable world problem. Here, we report a similar phenomenon in the tactile modality. When scanning a single Braille dot with two fingers of the same hand, participants were asked to decide whether the dot was stationary or whether it was displaced from one location to another. The stimulus was produced by refreshable Braille devices that have dots that can be swiftly raised and recessed. In some conditions, the dot was stationary. In others, a displacement was created by monitoring the participant’s finger position and by switching the dot activation when it was not touched by either finger. The dot displacement was of either 2.5 mm or 5 mm. We found that in certain cases, displaced dots were felt to be stationary. If the displacement was orthogonal to the finger movements, tactile suppression occurred effectively when it was of 2.5 mm, but when the displacement was of 5 mm, the participants easily detected it. If the displacement was medial–lateral, the suppression effect occurred as well, but less often when the apparent movement of the dot opposed the movement of the finger. In such cases, the stimulus appeared sooner than when the brain could predict it from finger movement, supporting a predictive rather than a postdictive differential processing hypothesis.

[1]  C. Ghez,et al.  Modulation of sensory transmission in cat lemniscal system during voluntary movement , 2004, Pflügers Archiv.

[2]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Self-motion and the perception of stationary objects , 2001, Nature.

[3]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[4]  S. Millar Reading by touch , 1997 .

[5]  Alexander H. Wertheim,et al.  The Direct versus Inferential controversy revisited , 2000 .

[6]  J. Craig,et al.  Grating orientation as a measure of tactile spatial acuity. , 1999, Somatosensory & motor research.

[7]  Paul M Bays,et al.  Spatial remapping of the visual world across saccades , 2007, Neuroreport.

[8]  R. Dodge Visual perception during eye movement , 1900 .

[9]  Marc O. Ernst,et al.  An instance of tactile suppression: Active exploration impairs tactile sensitivity for the direction of lateral movement , 2006 .

[10]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Time course and magnitude of movement-related gating of tactile detection in humans. III. Effect of motor tasks. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Tactile recognition of raised characters: A parametric study , 1985 .

[12]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Differential controls over tactile detection in humans by motor commands and peripheral reafference. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Attenuation of Self-Generated Tactile Sensations Is Predictive, not Postdictive , 2006, PLoS biology.

[14]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[15]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Time course and magnitude of movement-related gating of tactile detection in humans. I. Importance of stimulus location. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  J. Edeline,et al.  Evoked oscillations in the thalamo-cortical auditory system are present in anesthetized but not in unanesthetized rats. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  P. Haggard Sensory Neuroscience: From Skin to Object in the Somatosensory Cortex , 2006, Current Biology.

[18]  Robert K. Moore,et al.  Eyeblinks and visual suppression. , 1980, Science.

[19]  A. Noë,et al.  A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[20]  C E Chapman,et al.  Active versus passive touch: factors influencing the transmission of somatosensory signals to primary somatosensory cortex. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[21]  S. B. Stevenson,et al.  Dependence of visual suppression on the amplitudes of saccades and blinks , 1986, Vision Research.

[22]  Morton A. Heller,et al.  Active and passive tactile braille recognition , 1986 .

[23]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Perception of the Consequences of Self-Action Is Temporally Tuned and Event Driven , 2005, Current Biology.

[24]  Peter Thier,et al.  Modification of the filehne illusion by conditioning visual stimuli , 1996, Vision Research.

[25]  K. Cullen Sensory signals during active versus passive movement , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[26]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Common codes for situated interaction , 2001 .

[27]  Hong Z. Tan,et al.  When visual transients impair tactile change detection: A novel case of crossmodal change blindness? , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[28]  Henri Poincaré,et al.  Science et méthode , 1934 .

[29]  E. Matin Saccadic suppression: a review and an analysis. , 1974, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  D. Fleetwood,et al.  Non-volatile memory device based on mobile protons in SiO2 thin films , 1997, Nature.

[31]  L. Stark,et al.  Ocular proprioception and efference copy in registering visual direction , 1991, Vision Research.

[32]  H. Wallach,et al.  The effect of abnormal displacement of the retinal image during eye movements , 1966 .

[33]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Different effects of eyelid blinks and target blanking on saccadic suppression of displacement , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  R. Weale Why Does the Human Retina Possess a Fovea? , 1966, Nature.

[35]  Y. Hatwell,et al.  Toucher l'espace : la main et la perception tactile de l'espace , 1986 .

[36]  P. Haggard,et al.  Keeping the world a constant size: object constancy in human touch , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Michael S Vitevitch,et al.  Change deafness: the inability to detect changes between two voices. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  M. Merleau-Ponty Phénoménologie de la perception , 1950 .

[39]  T. Lenoir,et al.  Helmholtz and the Materialities of Communication , 1994, Osiris.

[40]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes , 1997 .

[41]  K O Johnson,et al.  Tactile spatial resolution. I. Two-point discrimination, gap detection, grating resolution, and letter recognition. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  John W. Lane,et al.  Receptive Field Properties of the Macaque Second Somatosensory Cortex: Representation of Orientation on Different Finger Pads , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Time course and magnitude of movement-related gating of tactile detection in humans. II. Effects of stimulus intensity. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.