Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort
暂无分享,去创建一个
M. Walton | M. Rushworth | D. Bannerman | T. Sharp | K. Jennings | D. Bannerman | M. F. S. Rushworth | M. Walton | T. Sharp | F. Denk | M. E. Walton | K. A. Jennings | D. M. Bannerman | F. Denk
[1] R. Wise,et al. A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. , 1987, Psychological review.
[2] T. Sharp,et al. Use of Arc expression as a molecular marker of increased postsynaptic 5‐HT function after SSRI/5‐HT1A receptor antagonist co‐administration , 2003, Journal of neurochemistry.
[3] Jack E. Henningfield,et al. Preference among research cigarettes with varying nicotine yields , 1990, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[4] P. Soubrié,et al. Benzodiazepines reduce the tolerance to reward delay in rats , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[5] J. Salamone,et al. Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions alter relative response allocation in a T-maze cost/benefit task , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.
[6] J. Evenden,et al. The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VI: the effects of ethanol and selective serotonergic drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement , 1999, Psychopharmacology.
[7] J. Deakin,et al. Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement , 2002, Psychopharmacology.
[8] Aaron Ettenberg,et al. Haloperidol blocks the response-reinstating effects of food reward: A methodology for separating neuroleptic effects on reinforcement and motor processes , 1988, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[9] J. Salamone,et al. Anhedonia or anergia? Effects of haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion on instrumental response selection in a T-maze cost/benefit procedure , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.
[10] T. Robbins,et al. Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] R. Wise. Neuroleptics and operant behavior: The anhedonia hypothesis , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[12] M. Hajós,et al. An electrophysiological and neuroanatomical study of the medial prefrontal cortical projection to the midbrain raphe nuclei in the rat , 1998, Neuroscience.
[13] Matthew F S Rushworth,et al. Functional Specialization within Medial Frontal Cortex of the Anterior Cingulate for Evaluating Effort-Related Decisions , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14] L. Acsády,et al. The effects of p-chlorophenylalanine-induced serotinin synthesis inhibition and muscarinic blockade on the performance of rats in a 5-choice serial reaction time task , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.
[15] T. Robbins,et al. Decision-making cognition in mania and depression , 2001, Psychological Medicine.
[16] M. Ernst,et al. Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[17] J. Salamone,et al. Nucleus accumbens dopamine and work requirements on interval schedules , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.
[18] C. M. Bradshaw,et al. Effect of lesions of the ascending 5-hydroxytryptaminergic pathways on choice between delayed reinforcers , 2005, Psychopharmacology.
[19] T. R. Wade,et al. Effects of dopaminergic drugs on delayed reward as a measure of impulsive behavior in rats , 2000, Psychopharmacology.
[20] T. Robbins,et al. Decision making and neuropsychiatry , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] Trevor W. Robbins,et al. Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction time task in rats: Implications for theories of selective attention and arousal , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.
[22] T. Sharp,et al. Release of cerebral 5-hydroxytryptamine evoked by electrical stimulation of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei: effect of a neurotoxic amphetamine , 1995, Neuroscience.
[23] T. Robbins,et al. Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[24] John D. Salamone,et al. Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions in rats affect relative response allocation in a novel cost/benefit procedure , 1994, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[25] J. Salamone,et al. Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions make rats more sensitive to high ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement , 1999, Neuroscience.
[26] M. Walton,et al. The Role of Rat Medial Frontal Cortex in Effort-Based Decision Making , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] S. Mobini,et al. Effects of central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement , 2000, Psychopharmacology.
[28] T. Robbins,et al. Impulsive Choice Induced in Rats by Lesions of the Nucleus Accumbens Core , 2001, Science.
[29] J. Evenden,et al. The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats: the effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement , 1996, Psychopharmacology.
[30] F. J. White,et al. Impaired DRL 30 performance during amphetamine withdrawal , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.
[31] J. Bizot,et al. Serotonin and tolerance to delay of reward in rats , 1999, Psychopharmacology.
[32] M. Walton,et al. The mesocortical dopamine projection to anterior cingulate cortex plays no role in guiding effort-related decisions. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.
[33] J. Salamone,et al. Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.
[34] A. Damasio,et al. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.
[35] T. Robbins,et al. Dissociable Deficits in the Decision-Making Cognition of Chronic Amphetamine Abusers, Opiate Abusers, Patients with Focal Damage to Prefrontal Cortex, and Tryptophan-Depleted Normal Volunteers: Evidence for Monoaminergic Mechanisms , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[36] C. Carter,et al. Tryptophan Depletion Alters the Decision-Making of Healthy Volunteers through Altered Processing of Reward Cues , 2003, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[37] J. Salamone,et al. Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions make animals highly sensitive to high fixed ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement , 2001, Neuroscience.
[38] J. Salamone,et al. Accumbens dopamine and the regulation of effort in food-seeking behavior: modulation of work output by different ratio or force requirements , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.
[39] P. Soubrié,et al. Effects of imipramine-like drugs and serotonin uptake blockers on delay of reward in rats. Possible implication in the behavioral mechanism of action of antidepressants. , 1988, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.
[40] Stephen C. Fowler,et al. Haloperidol produces within-session increments in operant response duration in rats , 1990, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[41] J. Evenden. Varieties of impulsivity , 1999, Psychopharmacology.
[42] J. Salamone,et al. Haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion suppress lever pressing for food but increase free food consumption in a novel food choice procedure , 2005, Psychopharmacology.
[43] M. Hajós,et al. A 5-hydroxytryptamine lesion markedly reduces the incidence of burst-firing dorsal raphe neurones in the rat , 1996, Neuroscience Letters.
[44] J. Salamone,et al. A microdialysis study of nucleus accumbens core and shell dopamine during operant responding in the rat , 1998, Neuroscience.
[45] C. M. Bradshaw,et al. Comparison of the effects of clozapine, haloperidol, chlorpromazine and d-amphetamine on performance on a time-constrained progressive ratio schedule and on locomotor behaviour in the rat , 2000, Psychopharmacology.
[46] J. Deakin,et al. Effects of quinolinic acid-induced lesions of the orbital prefrontal cortex on inter-temporal choice: a quantitative analysis , 2002, Psychopharmacology.