Differential involvement of serotonin and dopamine systems in cost-benefit decisions about delay or effort

RationaleAlthough tasks assessing the role of dopamine in effort-reward decisions are similar to those concerned with the role of serotonin in impulsive choice in that both require analysis of the costs and benefits of possible actions, they have never been directly compared.ObjectivesThis study investigated the involvement of serotonin and dopamine in two cost-benefit paradigms, one in which the cost was delay and the other in which it was physical effort.MethodsSixteen rats were trained on a T-maze task in which they chose between high and low reward arms. In one version, the high reward arm was obstructed by a barrier, in the other, delivery of the high reward was delayed by 15 s. Serotonin and dopamine function were manipulated using systemic pCPA and haloperidol injections, respectively.ResultsHaloperidol-treated rats were less inclined either to exert more effort or to countenance a delay for a higher reward. pCPA had no effect on the performance of the rats on the effortful task, but significantly increased the rats’ preference for an immediate but smaller reward. All animals (drug treated and controls) chose the high reward arm on the majority of trials when the delay or effort costs were matched in both high and low reward arms.ConclusionA dissociation was found between the neurotransmitter systems involved in different types of cost-benefit decision making. While dopaminergic systems were required for decisions about both effort and delay, serotonergic systems were only needed for the latter.

[1]  R. Wise,et al.  A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. , 1987, Psychological review.

[2]  T. Sharp,et al.  Use of Arc expression as a molecular marker of increased postsynaptic 5‐HT function after SSRI/5‐HT1A receptor antagonist co‐administration , 2003, Journal of neurochemistry.

[3]  Jack E. Henningfield,et al.  Preference among research cigarettes with varying nicotine yields , 1990, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[4]  P. Soubrié,et al.  Benzodiazepines reduce the tolerance to reward delay in rats , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[5]  J. Salamone,et al.  Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions alter relative response allocation in a T-maze cost/benefit task , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[6]  J. Evenden,et al.  The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VI: the effects of ethanol and selective serotonergic drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[7]  J. Deakin,et al.  Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement , 2002, Psychopharmacology.

[8]  Aaron Ettenberg,et al.  Haloperidol blocks the response-reinstating effects of food reward: A methodology for separating neuroleptic effects on reinforcement and motor processes , 1988, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[9]  J. Salamone,et al.  Anhedonia or anergia? Effects of haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion on instrumental response selection in a T-maze cost/benefit procedure , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[10]  T. Robbins,et al.  Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  R. Wise Neuroleptics and operant behavior: The anhedonia hypothesis , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  M. Hajós,et al.  An electrophysiological and neuroanatomical study of the medial prefrontal cortical projection to the midbrain raphe nuclei in the rat , 1998, Neuroscience.

[13]  Matthew F S Rushworth,et al.  Functional Specialization within Medial Frontal Cortex of the Anterior Cingulate for Evaluating Effort-Related Decisions , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  L. Acsády,et al.  The effects of p-chlorophenylalanine-induced serotinin synthesis inhibition and muscarinic blockade on the performance of rats in a 5-choice serial reaction time task , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[15]  T. Robbins,et al.  Decision-making cognition in mania and depression , 2001, Psychological Medicine.

[16]  M. Ernst,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  J. Salamone,et al.  Nucleus accumbens dopamine and work requirements on interval schedules , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[18]  C. M. Bradshaw,et al.  Effect of lesions of the ascending 5-hydroxytryptaminergic pathways on choice between delayed reinforcers , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[19]  T. R. Wade,et al.  Effects of dopaminergic drugs on delayed reward as a measure of impulsive behavior in rats , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[20]  T. Robbins,et al.  Decision making and neuropsychiatry , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Trevor W. Robbins,et al.  Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction time task in rats: Implications for theories of selective attention and arousal , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[22]  T. Sharp,et al.  Release of cerebral 5-hydroxytryptamine evoked by electrical stimulation of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei: effect of a neurotoxic amphetamine , 1995, Neuroscience.

[23]  T. Robbins,et al.  Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  John D. Salamone,et al.  Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions in rats affect relative response allocation in a novel cost/benefit procedure , 1994, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[25]  J. Salamone,et al.  Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions make rats more sensitive to high ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement , 1999, Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Walton,et al.  The Role of Rat Medial Frontal Cortex in Effort-Based Decision Making , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  S. Mobini,et al.  Effects of central 5-hydroxytryptamine depletion on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Impulsive Choice Induced in Rats by Lesions of the Nucleus Accumbens Core , 2001, Science.

[29]  J. Evenden,et al.  The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats: the effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement , 1996, Psychopharmacology.

[30]  F. J. White,et al.  Impaired DRL 30 performance during amphetamine withdrawal , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  J. Bizot,et al.  Serotonin and tolerance to delay of reward in rats , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[32]  M. Walton,et al.  The mesocortical dopamine projection to anterior cingulate cortex plays no role in guiding effort-related decisions. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.

[33]  J. Salamone,et al.  Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[35]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Deficits in the Decision-Making Cognition of Chronic Amphetamine Abusers, Opiate Abusers, Patients with Focal Damage to Prefrontal Cortex, and Tryptophan-Depleted Normal Volunteers: Evidence for Monoaminergic Mechanisms , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[36]  C. Carter,et al.  Tryptophan Depletion Alters the Decision-Making of Healthy Volunteers through Altered Processing of Reward Cues , 2003, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[37]  J. Salamone,et al.  Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions make animals highly sensitive to high fixed ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement , 2001, Neuroscience.

[38]  J. Salamone,et al.  Accumbens dopamine and the regulation of effort in food-seeking behavior: modulation of work output by different ratio or force requirements , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[39]  P. Soubrié,et al.  Effects of imipramine-like drugs and serotonin uptake blockers on delay of reward in rats. Possible implication in the behavioral mechanism of action of antidepressants. , 1988, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[40]  Stephen C. Fowler,et al.  Haloperidol produces within-session increments in operant response duration in rats , 1990, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[41]  J. Evenden Varieties of impulsivity , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[42]  J. Salamone,et al.  Haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion suppress lever pressing for food but increase free food consumption in a novel food choice procedure , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[43]  M. Hajós,et al.  A 5-hydroxytryptamine lesion markedly reduces the incidence of burst-firing dorsal raphe neurones in the rat , 1996, Neuroscience Letters.

[44]  J. Salamone,et al.  A microdialysis study of nucleus accumbens core and shell dopamine during operant responding in the rat , 1998, Neuroscience.

[45]  C. M. Bradshaw,et al.  Comparison of the effects of clozapine, haloperidol, chlorpromazine and d-amphetamine on performance on a time-constrained progressive ratio schedule and on locomotor behaviour in the rat , 2000, Psychopharmacology.

[46]  J. Deakin,et al.  Effects of quinolinic acid-induced lesions of the orbital prefrontal cortex on inter-temporal choice: a quantitative analysis , 2002, Psychopharmacology.