A 3-month intervention of Dance Dance Revolution improves interference control in elderly females: a preliminary investigation

Abstract Exercise regimens suitable to the elderly remain under investigated; therefore, this study examined the effects of Dance Dance Revolution (DDR) on cognitive control in elderly females. Twenty-six healthy elderly females leading a sedentary lifestyle were assigned to a DDR, brisk walking, or control group. Participants in the DDR and brisk walking groups engaged in moderate physical exercise three times per week for 3 months, whereas the control group maintained a sedentary lifestyle. Each participant performed a flanker task before and after the intervention. The results revealed that both DDR and brisk walking shortened reaction time, N2 latency, and P3 latency relative to those of the control group. These findings suggest that DDR intervention is as effective as that of brisk walking in improving inhibitory control for elderly people. Therefore, DDR can be used as a viable alternative exercise to enhance cognitive function for the elderly and motivate individuals who are less willing to be active.

[1]  A. Grace,et al.  The Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Polymorphism: Relations to the Tonic–Phasic Dopamine Hypothesis and Neuropsychiatric Phenotypes , 2004, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[2]  Catherine R. Harrison,et al.  Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function , 1999, Nature.

[3]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  Physical activity and cognitive function in a cross-section of younger and older community-dwelling individuals. , 2006, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[4]  Arthur B. Markman,et al.  Constraints on Analogical Inference , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  C. Cotman,et al.  Exercise and brain neurotrophins , 1995, Nature.

[6]  Jeffrey N. Browndyke,et al.  Aerobic Exercise and Neurocognitive Performance: A Meta-Analytic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials , 2010, Psychosomatic medicine.

[7]  T. McMorris,et al.  The effect of incremental exercise on cognitive performance. , 2000 .

[8]  C. Carter,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: An update of theory and data , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  A. Privat,et al.  Direct evidence for the link between monoaminergic descending pathways and motor activity. I. A study with microdialysis probes implanted in the ventral funiculus of the spinal cord , 1995, Brain Research.

[10]  C. Cotman,et al.  Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity , 2002, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  B. Resnick,et al.  Understanding what motivates older adults to exercise. , 2000, Journal of gerontological nursing.

[12]  Matthias Weippert,et al.  Comparison of three mobile devices for measuring R–R intervals and heart rate variability: Polar S810i, Suunto t6 and an ambulatory ECG system , 2010, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[13]  Cheryl L. Dickter,et al.  Strategic control and medial frontal negativity: beyond errors and response conflict. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[14]  Deanna M Barch,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of response inhibition: an fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  Aerobic fitness and neurocognitive function in healthy preadolescent children. , 2005, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[16]  J. Cummings,et al.  Effect of neuropsychiatric symptoms of Alzheimer's disease on Chinese and American caregivers , 2002, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[17]  F. Gage,et al.  Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[19]  Carl W. Cotman,et al.  Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades and inflammation , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[20]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[21]  Daniel C. Krawczyk,et al.  Structural constraints and object similarity in analogical mapping and inference , 2004 .

[22]  A. Kramer,et al.  Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults , 2003, Psychological science.

[23]  Didier Delignières,et al.  OPTIMAL AND NON-OPTIMAL DEMAND IN A DUAL TASK OF PEDALLING AND SIMPLE REACTION TIME: EFFECTS ON ENERGY EXPENDITURE AND COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE. , 1995 .

[24]  Bruce W Bailey,et al.  Energy cost of exergaming: a comparison of the energy cost of 6 forms of exergaming. , 2011, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[25]  C. Friedenreich,et al.  Sport participation in colorectal cancer survivors: an unexplored approach to promoting physical activity , 2012, Supportive Care in Cancer.

[26]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Risk-Sensitivity in Sensorimotor Control , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[27]  H. Jasper,et al.  The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. The International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. , 1999, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[28]  Stephanie Studenski,et al.  Interactive video dance games for healthy older adults , 2010, The journal of nutrition, health & aging.

[29]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[30]  Makoto Miyatani,et al.  A psychophysiological study on the function of the response‐stop in the Eriksen task1,2 , 2003 .

[31]  C. Hillman,et al.  Age, physical fitness, and attention: , 2009 .

[32]  A. Ortolani,et al.  Validation of a Polar® human heart rate monitor for measuring heart rate and heart rate variability in adult dogs under stationary conditions , 2012 .

[33]  W. Sinning,et al.  Y's Way to Physical Fitness: The Complete Guide to Fitness Testing and Instruction , 1989 .

[34]  Matthew B. Pontifex,et al.  Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: Event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[35]  I. Torres-Aleman,et al.  The effects of exercise on spatial learning and anxiety-like behavior are mediated by an IGF-I-dependent mechanism related to hippocampal neurogenesis , 2008, Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience.

[36]  Paige E. Scalf,et al.  Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. , 2006, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[37]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Novelty and conflict in the categorization of complex stimuli. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[38]  John E. Hummel,et al.  The One-to-One Constraint in Analogical Mapping and Inference , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[39]  C Bard,et al.  Effects of different types of physical activity on the performance of perceptual tasks in peripheral and central vision and coincident timing. , 1987, Ergonomics.

[40]  Jeroen Scheerder,et al.  Gender and age inequalities in regular sports participation: A cross-national study of 25 European countries , 2010, Journal of sports sciences.

[41]  Kim Delbaere,et al.  A Randomized Controlled Pilot Study of Home-Based Step Training in Older People Using Videogame Technology , 2013, PloS one.

[42]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Neurocognitive aging and cardiovascular fitness , 2007, Journal of Molecular Neuroscience.

[43]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  Age, physical fitness, and attention: P3a and P3b. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[44]  A. Miyake,et al.  The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[45]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Flanker interference in young and older participants as reflected in event-related potentials , 2008, Brain Research.

[46]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Disentangling Sequential Effects of Stimulus- and Response-related Conflict and Stimulus-Response Repetition using Brain Potentials , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Ben Godde,et al.  Human Neuroscience , 2009 .

[48]  John Sproule,et al.  Acute, intermediate intensity exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: A meta-analytical comparison of effects , 2011, Physiology & Behavior.

[49]  A. Privat,et al.  Direct evidence for the link between monoaminergic descending pathways and motor activity: II. A study with microdialysis probes implanted in the ventral horn of the spinal cord , 1998, Brain Research.

[50]  J. Chmura,et al.  Choice Reaction Time During Graded Exercise in Relation to Blood Lactate and Plasma Catecholamine Thresholds , 1994, International journal of sports medicine.

[51]  Katie Sell,et al.  Energy Expenditure During Physically Interactive Video Game Playing in Male College Students With Different Playing Experience , 2008, Journal of American college health : J of ACH.

[52]  H. Semlitsch,et al.  A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[53]  I. Torres-Aleman,et al.  Circulating Insulin-Like Growth Factor I Mediates the Protective Effects of Physical Exercise against Brain Insults of Different Etiology and Anatomy , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  R. Luke Oxygen cost and heart rate response during interactive whole body video gaming , 2005 .

[55]  A Kok,et al.  Varieties of inhibition: manifestations in cognition, event-related potentials and aging. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[56]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[57]  Markus Kiefer,et al.  Physical fitness, but not acute exercise modulates event-related potential indices for executive control in healthy adolescents , 2009, Brain Research.

[58]  J. Polich Updating P 300 : An Integrative Theory of P 3 a and P 3 b , 2009 .

[59]  Christopher R. Johnson,et al.  Cardiorespiratory Fitness and the Flexible Modulation of Cognitive Control in Preadolescent Children , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.