Involvement of vision in tool use in crow

Birds are capable of dexterous sensory–motor activities such as tool use. Reaching is a crucial component of tool use and is a vision-guided behavior in primates, in which arm movement is monitored online in a stable visual frame. However, vision-guided reaching in primates is enabled by anatomical separation of the head and arm; neck reaching in birds accompanies head movement, which produces unstable vision because the eye necessarily moves with the bill. This anatomical difference raises the question whether tool use in birds involves visuomotor mechanisms that are distinct from those in primates. As the role of vision in avian tool use has been poorly understood, we investigated the role of vision in tool use in the large-billed crow (Corvus macrorhynchos), a nontool user in the wild. Crows were trained to manipulate an L-shaped hook to retrieve food that was otherwise out of reach. After training, an opaque panel was placed on the front window of the platform to block their vision, and the effects on tool use were tested with respect to performance and movement trajectory. Vision blocking caused similar deviation of tool movement trajectories for both near and far targets, as well as far target-specific deviation. This suggests the involvement of vision in tool use by crows, specifically in the premanipulation process for conversion of vision-body coordinates for motor planning and in the process of tool manipulation. This is the first behavioral evidence for the involvement of vision in avian tool use.

[1]  R. Andersen,et al.  Cognitive neural prosthetics. , 2010, Annual review of psychology.

[2]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Shaping of Hooks in New Caledonian Crows , 2002, Science.

[3]  Jolyon Troscianko,et al.  Extreme binocular vision and a straight bill facilitate tool use in New Caledonian crows , 2012, Nature Communications.

[4]  Hans-Joachim Bischof,et al.  Eye movements of laterally eyed birds are not independent , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[5]  J. Hardin,et al.  Generalized Linear Models and Extensions , 2001 .

[6]  I. Schwab Vision, Brain, and Behavior in Birds , 1994 .

[7]  Nicola S. Clayton,et al.  Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius) , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[8]  V. Bentley-Condit,et al.  Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog , 2010 .

[9]  Martin Egelhaaf,et al.  Gaze Strategy in the Free Flying Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata) , 2008, PloS one.

[10]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Spontaneous innovation in tool manufacture and use in a Goffin’s cockatoo , 2012, Current Biology.

[11]  A. Iriki,et al.  Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: a scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  S. Schaal The Computational Neurobiology of Reaching and Pointing — A Foundation for Motor Learning by Reza Shadmehr and Steven P. Wise , 2007 .

[13]  G. Hunt,et al.  Tool-Making New Caledonian Crows Have Large Associative Brain Areas , 2010, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[14]  G. Hunt Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows , 1996, Nature.

[15]  C. Spence,et al.  Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans , 2002, Cognition.

[16]  Nathan J Emery,et al.  Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.