Reduced functional coupling in the default‐mode network during self‐referential processing

Activity within the default‐mode network (DMN) is thought to be related to self‐referential processing, such as thinking about one's preferences or personality traits. Although the DMN is generally considered to function as a network, evidence is starting to accumulate that suggests that areas of the DMN are each specialized for different subfunctions of self‐referential processing. Here, we address the issue of functional specialization by investigating changes in coupling between areas of the DMN during self‐referential processing. To this aim, brain activity was assessed during a task in which subjects had to indicate whether a trait adjective described their own personality (self‐referential, Self condition), that of another person (other‐referential, Other condition), or whether the trait was socially desirable (nonreferential, Control condition). To exclude confounding effects of cardiorespiratory processes on activity and functional coupling, we corrected the fMRI signal for these effects. Activity within areas of the DMN was found to be modulated by self‐referential processing. More specifically, during the Self condition compared to the Other and Control condition, activity within the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, ventral medial prefrontal cortex, and posterior cingulate cortex was increased. Moreover, coupling between areas of the DMN was reduced during the Self condition compared to the Other and Control condition, while coupling between regions of the DMN and regions outside the network was increased. As such, these results provide an indication for functional specialization within the DMN and support the notion that each area of the DMN is involved in different subfunctions of self‐referential processing. Hum Brain Mapp, 2010. © 2010 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  N. Anderson Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  F. Gonzalez-Lima,et al.  Structural equation modeling and its application to network analysis in functional brain imaging , 1994 .

[4]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[6]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses , 1997, Neuroreport.

[8]  M. Lowe,et al.  Functional Connectivity in Single and Multislice Echoplanar Imaging Using Resting-State Fluctuations , 1998, NeuroImage.

[9]  G. Winocur,et al.  In Search of the Self: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1999 .

[10]  G H Glover,et al.  Image‐based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[11]  J. Price,et al.  The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[13]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[14]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Neural correlates of self-reflection. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Troels W. Kjær,et al.  Reflective Self-Awareness and Conscious States: PET Evidence for a Common Midline Parietofrontal Core , 2002, NeuroImage.

[19]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The Anterior Frontomedian Cortex and Evaluative Judgment: An fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[20]  D. Amaral,et al.  Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: II. Cortical afferents , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modelling , 2003, NeuroImage.

[22]  F. Craik,et al.  In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. , 2003, The American journal of psychiatry.

[23]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  F. Bermpohl,et al.  Cortical midline structures and the self , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  Jane F. Banfield,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  G. Glover,et al.  Reflecting upon Feelings: An fMRI Study of Neural Systems Supporting the Attribution of Emotion to Self and Other , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Irene Tracey,et al.  Resting fluctuations in arterial carbon dioxide induce significant low frequency variations in BOLD signal , 2004, NeuroImage.

[29]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[30]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge , 2005, NeuroImage.

[31]  Martial Van der Linden,et al.  Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[32]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  S. Rombouts,et al.  Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Self-appraisal decisions evoke dissociated dorsal—ventral aMPFC networks , 2006, NeuroImage.

[35]  William M. Kelley,et al.  Neuroanatomical Evidence for Distinct Cognitive and Affective Components of Self , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[37]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[38]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[39]  Peter A. Bandettini,et al.  Separating respiratory-variation-related fluctuations from neuronal-activity-related fluctuations in fMRI , 2006, NeuroImage.

[40]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulate activity during self-reflection. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[41]  Perrine Ruby,et al.  Distinct Regions of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Are Associated with Self-referential Processing and Perspective Taking , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Jeff H. Duyn,et al.  Low-frequency fluctuations in the cardiac rate as a source of variance in the resting-state fMRI BOLD signal , 2007, NeuroImage.

[43]  D. Amaral,et al.  Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: III. Cortical efferents , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[44]  R. Poldrack Region of interest analysis for fMRI. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[45]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[46]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Relevance to self: A brief review and framework of neural systems underlying appraisal , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[47]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[48]  M. P. van den Heuvel,et al.  Microstructural Organization of the Cingulum Tract and the Level of Default Mode Functional Connectivity , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  H. Heinze,et al.  The resting brain and our self: Self-relatedness modulates resting state neural activity in cortical midline structures , 2008, Neuroscience.

[50]  B. Biswal,et al.  Network homogeneity reveals decreased integrity of default-mode network in ADHD , 2008, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[51]  M. Greicius,et al.  Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  Matthijs Vink,et al.  Cardiorespiratory effects on default‐mode network activity as measured with fMRI , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[53]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dynamic causal modeling , 2010, Scholarpedia.