Mapping Words to the World in Infancy: Infants' Expectations for Count Nouns and Adjectives

Three experimentsdocumentthat 14-month-old infants'construal of objects (e.g., purple animals) is influenced by naming, that they can distinguish between the grammatical form noun and adjective, and that they treat this distinction as relevant to meaning. In each experiment, infants extended novel nouns (e.g., "This one is a blicket") specifically to object categories (e.g., animal), and not to object properties (e.g., purple things). This robust noun-category link is related to grammatical form and not to surface differences in the presentation of novelwords (Experiment 3). Infants'extensions of novel adjectives (e.g., "This one is blickish") were more fragile: They extended adjectives specifically to object properties when the property was color (Experiment 1), but revealed a less precise mapping when the property was texture (Experiment 2). These results reveal that by 14 months, infants distinguish between grammatical forms and utilize these distinctions in determining the meaning of novel words.

[1]  S. Waxman,et al.  Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories for infants. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[2]  E. Markman,et al.  The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning: By W. E. Merriman and L. L. Bowman. In Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54, Serial No. 220, 1989☆ , 1991 .

[3]  Toben H. Mintz,et al.  Adjectives really do modify nouns: the incremental and restricted nature of early adjective acquisition , 2002, Cognition.

[4]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[5]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Variability in early communicative development. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[6]  B. Lust,et al.  Syntactic theory and first language acquisition : cross-linguistic perspectives , 1997 .

[7]  T. Wilcox Object individuation: infants’ use of shape, size, pattern, and color , 1999, Cognition.

[8]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Color vision and hue categorization in young human infants. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Sandra R Waxman,et al.  The role of comparison in the extension of novel adjectives. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[10]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Early word meanings: The case of object names , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  The origins and evolution of links between word learning and conceptual organization: new evidence from 11‐month‐olds , 2003 .

[12]  S. Waxman,et al.  Object properties and object kind: twenty-one-month-old infants' extension of novel adjectives. , 1998, Child development.

[13]  J. Werker,et al.  Newborn infants’ sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words , 1999, Cognition.

[14]  Brian MacWhinney,et al.  The acquisition of grammar , 1986 .

[15]  S. Waxman,et al.  Basic level object categories support the acquisition of novel adjectives: evidence from preschool-aged children. , 2000, Child development.

[16]  E. Markman,et al.  Young children's inductions from natural kinds: the role of categories and appearances. , 1987, Child development.

[17]  S. Waxman,et al.  The development of a linkage between count nouns and object categories: evidence from fifteen- to twenty-one-month-old infants. , 1993, Child development.

[18]  W. Merriman,et al.  Restricting a familiar name in response to learning a new one: evidence for the mutual exclusivity bias in young two-year-olds. , 1997, Child development.

[19]  W. Merriman,et al.  The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[20]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Language learnability and language development , 1985 .

[21]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Early Word‐Learning and Conceptual Development: Everything had a Name, and Each Name Gave Birth to a New Thought , 2007 .

[22]  S. Waxman,et al.  Nouns mark category relations: toddlers' and preschoolers' word-learning biases. , 1990, Child development.

[23]  E. Markman,et al.  Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning , 1994 .

[24]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  The Emergence of Category Representations During Infancy: Are Separate Perceptual and Conceptual Processes Required? , 2000 .

[25]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  How Two‐ and Four‐Year‐Old Children Interpret Adjectives and Count Nouns , 1993 .

[26]  E. Markman,et al.  Implicit contrast in adjectives vs. nouns: implications for word-learning in preschoolers , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[27]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Syntactic context and the shape bias in children's and adults' lexical learning , 1992 .

[28]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Early noun vocabularies: do ontology, category structure and syntax correspond? , 1999, Cognition.

[29]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[30]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  A Cross-Linguistic Examination of the Noun-Category Bias: Its Existence and Specificity in French- and Spanish-Speaking Preschool-Aged Children , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Specifying the scope of 13-month-olds' expectations for novel words , 1999, Cognition.

[32]  R. Dixon Where Have all the Adjectives Gone , 1977 .

[33]  L. Talmy Lexicalisation patterns: semantic structure in lexical forms , 1985 .

[34]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .

[35]  Susan A Gelman,et al.  Shape and representational status in children's early naming , 1998, Cognition.

[36]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[37]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  On the development of color naming in young children: Data and theory , 1985, Brain and Language.

[38]  J. Mandler Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Infancy , 2000 .

[39]  B. J. Winer,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, 2nd Edition. , 1973 .

[40]  L. Gleitman The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.

[41]  A. Wierzbicka Does language reflect culture? Evidence from Australian English , 1986, Language in Society.

[42]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Linguistic biases and the establishment of conceptual hierarchies: Evidence from preschool children , 1990 .

[43]  J. Macnamara Names for Things: A Study in Human Learning , 1984 .

[44]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  Development of the First Language in the Human Species. , 1973 .

[45]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. , 1996, Child development.

[46]  S. Waxman The dubbing ceremony revisited: Object naming and categorization in infancy and early childhood , 1999 .

[47]  D G Hall,et al.  Semantic constraints on word learning: proper names and adjectives. , 1994, Child development.

[48]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .

[49]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Establishing word-object relations: a first step. , 1989, Child development.

[50]  K. Nelson,et al.  Structure and strategy in learning to talk. , 1973 .

[51]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Seeing Pink Elephants: Fourteen-Month-Olds' Interpretations of Novel Nouns and Adjectives , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[52]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Assumptions about Word Meaning: Individuation and Basic‐Level Kinds , 1993 .