Pitch-class distribution modulates the statistical learning of atonal chord sequences
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Stefan Koelsch,et al. Under the hood of statistical learning: A statistical MMN reflects the magnitude of transitional probabilities in auditory sequences , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[2] S. Koelsch,et al. The effects of supervised learning on event-related potential correlates of music-syntactic processing , 2015, Brain Research.
[3] Masato Yumoto,et al. Statistical learning of music- and language-like sequences and tolerance for spectral shifts , 2015, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[4] Clara E. James,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for a specific neural correlate of musical violation expectation in primary-school children , 2015, NeuroImage.
[5] M. Yumoto,et al. Implicit and explicit statistical learning of tone sequences across spectral shifts , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[6] D. Schön,et al. Neural sensitivity to statistical regularities as a fundamental biological process that underlies auditory learning: The role of musical practice , 2014, Hearing Research.
[7] Hubert Preissl,et al. Modulations of neural activity in auditory streaming caused by spectral and temporal alternation in subsequent stimuli: a magnetoencephalographic study , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.
[8] Sibylle C. Herholz,et al. Statistical learning effects in musicians and non-musicians: An MEG study , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[9] Daniele Schön,et al. Musical Expertise and Statistical Learning of Musical and Linguistic Structures , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[10] Seung-Goo Kim,et al. The Effect of Conditional Probability of Chord Progression on Brain Response: An MEG Study , 2011, PloS one.
[11] John Shawe-Taylor,et al. Neural prediction of higher-order auditory sequence statistics , 2011, NeuroImage.
[12] Daniele Schön,et al. Learning of musical and linguistic structures: comparing event-related potentials and behavior , 2010, Neuroreport.
[13] Jenny R. Saffran,et al. Learning Harmony: The Role of Serial Statistics , 2009, Cogn. Sci..
[14] Riitta Hari,et al. Removal of magnetoencephalographic artifacts with temporal signal‐space separation: Demonstration with single‐trial auditory‐evoked responses , 2009, Human brain mapping.
[15] Mitchell Steinschneider,et al. Effects of musical training on sound pattern processing in high-school students. , 2009, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.
[16] Lisa D. Sanders,et al. Event-related potentials index segmentation of nonsense sounds , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[17] B. Ross,et al. Stimulus experience modifies auditory neuromagnetic responses in young and older listeners , 2009, Hearing Research.
[18] S. Koelsch. Music-syntactic processing and auditory memory: similarities and differences between ERAN and MMN. , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[19] Pedro M. Domingos,et al. Joint Unsupervised Coreference Resolution with Markov Logic , 2008, EMNLP.
[20] Kazuo Okanoya,et al. On-line Assessment of Statistical Learning by Event-related Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] Isabelle Peretz,et al. Songs as an aid for language acquisition , 2008, Cognition.
[22] A. Whittall. The Cambridge introduction to serialism , 2008 .
[23] Pedro M. Domingos,et al. Joint Inference in Information Extraction , 2007, AAAI.
[24] Pedro M. Domingos,et al. Entity Resolution with Markov Logic , 2006, Sixth International Conference on Data Mining (ICDM'06).
[25] R. Karlıdağ,et al. Reduced P50 auditory sensory gating response in professional musicians , 2006, Brain and Cognition.
[26] S. Trehub,et al. Infant music perception: Domain-general or domain-specific mechanisms? , 2006, Cognition.
[27] R. Jackendoff,et al. The capacity for music: What is it, and what’s special about it? , 2006, Cognition.
[28] Matthew Richardson,et al. Markov logic networks , 2006, Machine Learning.
[29] D. Vernon,et al. Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates of Human Auditory Sensory Memory-Trace Formation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[30] Claude Alain,et al. I've heard it all before: perceptual invariance represented by early cortical auditory-evoked responses. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[31] M. Kisley,et al. Comparison of sensory gating to mismatch negativity and self-reported perceptual phenomena in healthy adults. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[32] M. Coltheart,et al. Modularity of music processing , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] Noam Chomsky,et al. The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002, Science.
[34] I. Peretz,et al. Congenital amusia: a group study of adults afflicted with a music-specific disorder. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[35] J. Saffran,et al. Infant Long‐Term Memory for Music , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[36] O Bertrand,et al. Multiple supratemporal sources of magnetic and electric auditory evoked middle latency components in humans. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[37] Barbara Tillmann,et al. Implicit learning of tonality: A self-organizing approach , 2000 .
[38] D. Javitt. Intracortical Mechanisms of Mismatch Negativity Dysfunction in Schizophrenia , 2000, Audiology and Neurotology.
[39] A. Friederici,et al. Brain Indices of Music Processing: Nonmusicians are Musical , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] E. Gibson,et al. The P600 as an index of syntactic integration difficulty , 2000 .
[41] B. Tillmann,et al. Implicit learning of tonality: a self-organizing approach. , 2000, Psychological review.
[42] A. Belger,et al. Midlatency evoked potentials attenuation and augmentation reflect different aspects of sensory gating , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.
[43] Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.
[44] Aniruddh D. Patel,et al. Processing Syntactic Relations in Language and Music: An Event-Related Potential Study , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] M. Goldsmith,et al. Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .
[46] A. Friederici. The Time Course of Syntactic Activation During Language Processing: A Model Based on Neuropsychological and Neurophysiological Data , 1995, Brain and Language.
[47] P. Tueting,et al. The P50 evoked potential component and mismatch detection in normal volunteers: implications for the study of sensory gating , 1995, Psychiatry Research.
[48] J M Badier,et al. Evoked potentials recorded from the auditory cortex in man: evaluation and topography of the middle latency components. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[49] A. Friederici,et al. Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[50] L. Kaufman,et al. Human auditory primary and association cortex have differing lifetimes for activation traces , 1992, Brain Research.
[51] J. D. Smith,et al. Aesthetic preference and syntactic prototypicality in music: 'Tis the gift to be simple , 1990, Cognition.
[52] J S Buchwald,et al. Midlatency auditory evoked responses: differential recovery cycle characteristics. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[53] C. Krumhansl,et al. Tonal hierarchies in the music of north India. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[54] Robert B. Cantrick,et al. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music , 1985 .
[55] R Freedman,et al. Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in neuronal mechanisms involved in sensory gating in schizophrenia. , 1982, Biological psychiatry.
[56] M. Kutas,et al. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.
[57] Allan KElLER,et al. BERNSTEIN'S THE UNANSWERED QUESTION AND THE PROBLEM OF MUSICAL COMPETENCE , 1978 .
[58] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.