Prefrontal Serotonin Depletion Affects Reversal Learning But Not Attentional Set Shifting

Recently, we have shown that serotonin (5-HT) depletion from the prefrontal cortex (PFC) of the marmoset monkey impairs performance on a serial discrimination reversal (SDR) task, resulting in perseverative responding to the previously correct stimulus (Clarke et al., 2004). This pattern of impairment is just one example of inflexible responding seen after damage to the PFC, with performance on the SDR task being dependent on the integrity of the orbitofrontal cortex. However, the contribution of 5-HT to other forms of flexible responding, such as attentional set shifting, an ability dependent on lateral PFC (Dias et al., 1996a), is unknown. The present study addresses this issue by examining the effects of 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine-induced PFC 5-HT depletions on the ability to shift attention between two perceptual dimensions of a compound visual stimulus (extradimensional shift). Monkeys with selective PFC 5-HT lesions, despite being impaired in their ability to reverse a stimulus-reward association, were unimpaired in their ability to make an extradimensional shift when compared with sham-operated controls. These findings suggest that 5-HT is critical for flexible responding at the level of changing stimulus-reward contingencies but is not essential for the higher-order shifting of attentional set. Thus, psychological functions dependent on different loci within the PFC are differentially sensitive to serotonergic modulation, a finding of relevance to our understanding of cognitive inflexibility apparent in disorders such as obsessive-compulsive disorder and schizophrenia.

[1]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[2]  Z. Šidák Rectangular Confidence Regions for the Means of Multivariate Normal Distributions , 1967 .

[3]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus--reinforcement associations. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[4]  G. Keppel,et al.  Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 1976 .

[5]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. , 1979, Science.

[6]  K. Brodmann Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde : in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues , 1985 .

[7]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. II. Role of neuroleptic treatment, attention, and mental effort. , 1986, Archives of general psychiatry.

[8]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. , 1986, Archives of general psychiatry.

[9]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[10]  Frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1987 .

[11]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[12]  H. Meltzer Serotonergic Dysfunction in Depression , 1989, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[13]  D L Braff,et al.  Increased distractibility in schizophrenic patients. Electrophysiologic and behavioral evidence. , 1990, Archives of general psychiatry.

[14]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[15]  T. Robbins,et al.  A specific form of cognitive rigidity following excitotoxic lesions of the basal forebrain in marmosets , 1992, Neuroscience.

[16]  T. Goldberg,et al.  The Effect of Clozapine on Cognition and Psychiatric Symptoms in Patients with Schizophrenia , 1993, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[17]  L. Friedman,et al.  Improvement in cognitive functions and psychiatric symptoms in treatment-refractory schizophrenic patients receiving clozapine , 1993, Biological Psychiatry.

[18]  E. Rolls,et al.  Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[19]  J. T. Coull,et al.  Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces selective impairments in learning and memory , 1994, Neuropharmacology.

[20]  A C Roberts,et al.  6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys enhance performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: possible interactions with subcortical dopamine , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  H. Meltzer,et al.  Effects of clozapine on cognitive function in schizophrenia. , 1994, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[22]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for frontostriatal dysfunction in schizophrenia , 1995, Psychological Medicine.

[23]  A C Roberts,et al.  Primate analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[24]  J. Zohar,et al.  Paroxetine versus Clomipramine in the Treatment of Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder , 1996, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[25]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[26]  R Morice,et al.  Frontal/executive impairments in schizophrenia. , 1996, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[27]  R. Oades Stimulus dimension shifts in patients with schizophrenia, with and without paranoid hallucinatory symptoms, or obsessive compulsive disorder: strategies, blocking and monoamine status , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Forms of Inhibitory Control within Prefrontal Cortex with an Analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: Restriction to Novel Situations and Independence from “On-Line” Processing , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  S. Scarone,et al.  The selective breakdown of frontal functions in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder and in patients with schizophrenia: A double dissociation experimental finding , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  J. Steere,et al.  The alpha-2A noradrenergic receptor agonist guanfacine improves visual object discrimination reversal performance in aged rhesus monkeys. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[31]  J. Steere,et al.  The α-2A noradrenergic receptor agonist guanfacine improves visual object discrimination reversal performance in aged rhesus monkeys. , 1997 .

[32]  S. Maier,et al.  Exposure to inescapable but not escapable shock increases extracellular levels of 5-HT in the dorsal raphe nucleus of the rat , 1998, Brain Research.

[33]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Noise stress impairs prefrontal cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism. , 1998, Archives of general psychiatry.

[34]  M Freedman,et al.  Orbitofrontal function, object alternation and perseveration. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  C. Montigny,et al.  Possible serotonergic mechanisms underlying the antidepressant and anti-obsessive–compulsive disorder responses , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[36]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Idazoxan potentiates rather than antagonizes some of the cognitive effects of clonidine , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[37]  Bao-Ming Li,et al.  Local infusion of an α-1 adrenergic agonist into the prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance in monkeys , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[38]  M. Thase,et al.  Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in depression determined by Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[39]  T. Robbins,et al.  Choosing between Small, Likely Rewards and Large, Unlikely Rewards Activates Inferior and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  T. Robbins,et al.  Comparison of set-shifting ability in patients with chronic schizophrenia and frontal lobe damage , 1999, Schizophrenia Research.

[41]  T. Robbins,et al.  Systemic sulpiride in young adult volunteers simulates the profile of cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[42]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Deficits in the Decision-Making Cognition of Chronic Amphetamine Abusers, Opiate Abusers, Patients with Focal Damage to Prefrontal Cortex, and Tryptophan-Depleted Normal Volunteers: Evidence for Monoaminergic Mechanisms , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[43]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting Cortical and Subcortical Activations Produced by Attentional-Set Shifting and Reversal Learning in Humans , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  T. Robbins,et al.  Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  H. McFarlane,et al.  M100,907, a selective 5-HT2A antagonist, attenuates dopamine release in the rat medial prefrontal cortex , 2001, Brain Research.

[46]  H. Meltzer,et al.  5‐HT2A and D2 receptor blockade increases cortical DA release via 5‐HT1A receptor activation: a possible mechanism of atypical antipsychotic‐induced cortical dopamine release , 2001, Journal of neurochemistry.

[47]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  The Physiological Role of 5-HT2A Receptors in Working Memory , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  D. Stein,et al.  Investigating the role of dopaminergic and serotonergic candidate genes in obsessive-compulsive disorder , 2003, European Neuropsychopharmacology.

[49]  Morten L Kringelbach,et al.  Neural correlates of rapid reversal learning in a simple model of human social interaction , 2003, NeuroImage.

[50]  U. Hadar,et al.  Perseveration and over-switching in schizophrenia , 2003, Schizophrenia Research.

[51]  D. Grandy,et al.  Dopamine D2 receptors mediate two-odor discrimination and reversal learning in C57BL/6 mice , 2004, BMC Neuroscience.

[52]  M. Farah,et al.  Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[53]  Mary E Stewart,et al.  The Effects of Acute Tryptophan Depletion on Neuropsychological Function , 2003, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[54]  M. Caron,et al.  Center stage for the serotonin transporter: a gain-of-function polymorphism in persons with obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2003, Molecular pharmacology.

[55]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Dissociating Valence of Outcome from Behavioral Control in Human Orbital and Ventral Prefrontal Cortices , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  C. Carter,et al.  Tryptophan Depletion Alters the Decision-Making of Healthy Volunteers through Altered Processing of Reward Cues , 2003, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[57]  Yasuhiro Kaneda,et al.  Serotonin receptors : their key role in drugs to treat schizophrenia , 2003, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[58]  M. Brammer,et al.  Neural correlates of switching set as measured in fast, event‐related functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[59]  Paul J. Harrison,et al.  Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Inhibition Improves Set-Shifting Performance and Elevates Stimulated Dopamine Release in the Rat Prefrontal Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  M. Baxter,et al.  Central blockade of muscarinic cholinergic receptors disrupts affective and attentional set‐shifting , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[61]  David W. Evans,et al.  The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in normally developing compulsive-like behaviors and obsessive–compulsive disorder , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[62]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive Inflexibility After Prefrontal Serotonin Depletion , 2004, Science.

[63]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Clonidine and diazepam have differential effects on tests of attention and learning , 1995, Psychopharmacology.