The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Maurice Merleau-Ponty. Phenomenology of Perception , 1964 .
[2] R B Welch,et al. An examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] D. C. Howell. Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .
[4] D. C. Howell. Statistical methods for psychology, 3rd ed. , 1992 .
[5] Xiaoqin Wang,et al. Remodelling of hand representation in adult cortex determined by timing of tactile stimulation , 1995, Nature.
[6] J. Gordon,et al. Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] M. Tanaka,et al. Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[8] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.
[9] V. Ramachandran,et al. The perception of phantom limbs , 1998 .
[10] V. Ramachandran,et al. The perception of phantom limbs. The D. O. Hebb lecture. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] M. Graziano. Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[12] Robert B. Welch,et al. How Can We Determine if the Sense of Presence Affects Task Performance? , 1999, Presence.
[13] C Rorden,et al. When a rubber hand 'feels' what the real hand cannot. , 1999, Neuroreport.
[14] C. Spence,et al. Visual Capture of Touch: Out-of-the-Body Experiences With Rubber Gloves , 2000, Psychological science.
[15] A. Berti,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[16] A Farnè,et al. Dynamic size‐change of hand peripersonal space following tool use , 2000, Neuroreport.
[17] M S Graziano,et al. Coding the location of the arm by sight. , 2000, Science.
[18] F. Pavani,et al. Left tactile extinction following visual stimulation of a rubber hand. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[19] C Braun,et al. Activity patterns of human somatosensory cortex adapt dynamically to stimulus properties , 2000, Neuroreport.
[20] C Braun,et al. Differential Activation in Somatosensory Cortex for Different Discrimination Tasks , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] P. Rochat,et al. Perceived self in infancy , 2000 .
[22] S. Kitazawa,et al. Sensation at the tips of invisible tools , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] S. Kitazawa,et al. Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] P. Haggard,et al. Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans , 2001, Current Biology.
[25] S. Tipper,et al. Vision influences tactile perception at body sites that cannot be viewed directly , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[26] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance Volume Xix , 2001 .
[27] M. Jeannerod,et al. Sense of body and sense of action both contribute to self-recognition , 2002, Cognition.
[28] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[29] C. Spence,et al. Seeing Your Own Touched Hands in a Mirror Modulates Cross-Modal Interactions , 2002, Psychological science.
[30] Larry E. Roberts,et al. Functional Organization of Primary Somatosensory Cortex Depends on the Focus of Attention , 2002, NeuroImage.
[31] V. Ramachandran,et al. Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[32] C. Spence,et al. Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach , 2003, Current Biology.
[33] P. Haggard,et al. Tactile perception, cortical representation and the bodily self , 2003, Current Biology.
[34] Patrick Haggard,et al. Experimenting with the acting self , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.