Eye tracking to evaluate evidence recognition in crime scene investigations.

Crime scene analysts are the core of criminal investigations; decisions made at the scene greatly affect the speed of analysis and the quality of conclusions, thereby directly impacting the successful resolution of a case. If an examiner fails to recognize the pertinence of an item on scene, the analyst's theory regarding the crime will be limited. Conversely, unselective evidence collection will most likely include irrelevant material, thus increasing a forensic laboratory's backlog and potentially sending the investigation into an unproductive and costly direction. Therefore, it is critical that analysts recognize and properly evaluate forensic evidence that can assess the relative support of differing hypotheses related to event reconstruction. With this in mind, the aim of this study was to determine if quantitative eye tracking data and qualitative reconstruction accuracy could be used to distinguish investigator expertise. In order to assess this, 32 participants were successfully recruited and categorized as experts or trained novices based on their practical experiences and educational backgrounds. Each volunteer then processed a mock crime scene while wearing a mobile eye tracker, wherein visual fixations, durations, search patterns, and reconstruction accuracy were evaluated. The eye tracking data (dwell time and task percentage on areas of interest or AOIs) were compared using Earth Mover's Distance (EMD) and the Needleman-Wunsch (N-W) algorithm, revealing significant group differences for both search duration (EMD), as well as search sequence (N-W). More specifically, experts exhibited greater dissimilarity in search duration, but greater similarity in search sequences than their novice counterparts. In addition to the quantitative visual assessment of examiner variability, each participant's reconstruction skill was assessed using a 22-point binary scoring system, in which significant group differences were detected as a function of total reconstruction accuracy. This result, coupled with the fact that the study failed to detect a significant difference between the groups when evaluating the total time needed to complete the investigation, indicates that experts are more efficient and effective. Finally, the results presented here provide a basis for continued research in the use of eye trackers to assess expertise in complex and distributed environments, including suggestions for future work, and cautions regarding the degree to which visual attention can infer cognitive understanding.

[1]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Cheating experience: Guiding novices to adopt the gaze strategies of experts expedites the learning of technical laparoscopic skills. , 2012, Surgery.

[2]  Guang-Zhong Yang,et al.  Analysis of visual search patterns with EMD metric in normalized anatomical space , 2006, IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging.

[3]  Karen M. Evans,et al.  Collecting and Analyzing Eye-Tracking Data in Outdoor Environments , 2012 .

[4]  E. Krupinski,et al.  Eye-movement study and human performance using telepathology virtual slides: implications for medical education and differences with experience. , 2006, Human pathology.

[5]  Bryan Found,et al.  Visual Attention and Expertise for Forensic Signature Analysis , 2006, Journal of forensic sciences.

[6]  Bryan F. J. Manly,et al.  Multivariate Statistical Methods : A Primer , 1986 .

[7]  Junle Wang,et al.  Study of depth bias of observers in free viewing of still stereoscopic synthetic stimuli , 2012 .

[8]  Andrew T Duchowski,et al.  A breadth-first survey of eye-tracking applications , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[9]  Olivier Delémont,et al.  Utility of the clue - From assessing the investigative contribution of forensic science to supporting the decision to use traces. , 2015, Science & justice : journal of the Forensic Science Society.

[10]  Chen Yu,et al.  Consistency and Variability Among Latent Print Examiners as Revealed by Eye Tracking Methodologies , 2011 .

[11]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Gaze training improves the retention and transfer of laparoscopic technical skills in novices , 2013, Surgical Endoscopy.

[12]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  ScanMatch: A novel method for comparing fixation sequences , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[13]  Guang-Zhong Yang,et al.  Differences in gaze behaviour of expert and junior surgeons performing open inguinal hernia repair , 2015, Surgical Endoscopy.

[14]  Tim Schmitz Encyclopedia Of Law Enforcement , 2016 .

[15]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Psychomotor control in a virtual laparoscopic surgery training environment: gaze control parameters differentiate novices from experts , 2010, Surgical Endoscopy.

[16]  Katarzyna Harezlak,et al.  Towards Accurate Eye Tracker Calibration - Methods and Procedures , 2014, KES.

[17]  Richard G. Brereton,et al.  Chemometrics for Pattern Recognition , 2009 .

[18]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Advances in the application of information technology to sport performance , 2002, Journal of sports sciences.

[19]  Durdica Hazard,et al.  The Relevant Physical Trace in Criminal Investigation , 2016 .

[20]  Matthieu Lenoir,et al.  Measuring dwell time percentage from head-mounted eye-tracking data – comparison of a frame-by-frame and a fixation-by-fixation analysis , 2015, Ergonomics.

[21]  Hiroyuki Hamada,et al.  Influence of Proficiency on Eye Movement of the Surgeon for Laparoscopic Cholecystectomy , 2015, HCI.

[22]  Jonathan Denniss,et al.  Examination strategies of experienced and novice clinicians viewing the retina , 2015, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[23]  Law. Policy Executive Summary of the National Academies of Science Reports, Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward , 2009 .