Head roll influences perceived hand position.

Visual and proprioceptive sensory inputs are naturally coded in different reference frames, i.e., eye-centered and body-centered, respectively. To use these signals in conjunction for motor planning or perception ultimately requires converting them into a common frame of reference using estimates of the relative orientation of the eyes, head, and body. Here, we examine whether extraretinal signals-specifically head roll-alter multisensory perception through noisy reference frame transformations. To do so, we examine the accuracy of visual localization relative to proprioceptive hand position for different head roll orientations. Subjects were required to judge whether a visual target was located closer or further and left or right (4-alternative forced-choice task) from their unseen hand. This was done for three different head roll rotations (-30, 0, and 30 deg). We show that eccentric head roll increased the variability in the subjects' ability to discriminate target location relative to the fingertip. We conclude that sensory perception is sensitive to body-geometry-dependent noise affecting the coordinate matching transformations of sensory data.

[1]  S. Lechner-Steinleitner,et al.  Interaction of labyrinthine and somatoreceptor inputs as determinants of the subjective vertical , 1978, Psychological research.

[2]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Oculocentric frames of reference for limb movement. , 2002, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[3]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Multisensory Integration during Motor Planning , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[5]  D. Knill,et al.  The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[6]  Yale E. Cohen,et al.  A common reference frame for movement plans in the posterior parietal cortex , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  J F Soechting,et al.  Moving in three-dimensional space: frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  L. Harris,et al.  The subjective visual vertical and the perceptual upright , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  I. Curthoys,et al.  The Effect of Ocular Torsional Position on Perception of the Roll-tilt of Visual Stimuli , 1997, Vision Research.

[11]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Errors in pointing are due to approximations in sensorimotor transformations. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  A. Faisal,et al.  Noise in the nervous system , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[15]  W Pieter Medendorp,et al.  Rotational Remapping in Human Spatial Memory during Eye and Head Motion , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Hans-Ulrich Dodt,et al.  Shining Light on Neurons - Elucidation of Neuronal Functions by Photostimulation , 2004, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[17]  R F Lewis,et al.  Efference copy provides the eye position information required for visually guided reaching. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Computations for geometrically accurate visually guided reaching in 3-D space. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[19]  M Guerraz,et al.  The role of neck afferents in subjective orientation in the visual and tactile sensory modalities. , 2000, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[20]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Role of uncertainty in sensorimotor control. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[21]  Christina T Fuentes,et al.  Where is your arm? Variations in proprioception across space and tasks. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  F. J. Clark,et al.  A metric for assessing acuity in positioning joints and limbs , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  E I Knudsen,et al.  Computational maps in the brain. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  D Straumann,et al.  Roll-dependent modulation of the subjective visual vertical: contributions of head- and trunk-based signals. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  G E Loeb,et al.  The computation of position sense from spindles in mono- and multiarticular muscles , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  R. Andersen,et al.  The posterior parietal cortex: Sensorimotor interface for the planning and online control of visually guided movements , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  D Straumann,et al.  Gravity dependence of subjective visual vertical variability. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Michele Tagliabue,et al.  Necessity is the Mother of Invention: Reconstructing Missing Sensory Information in Multiple, Concurrent Reference Frames for Eye–Hand Coordination , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Roles of gravitational cues and efference copy signals in the rotational updating of memory saccades. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  M. Chacron,et al.  Neural Variability, Detection Thresholds, and Information Transmission in the Vestibular System , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  W P Medendorp,et al.  Fusion of visual and vestibular tilt cues in the perception of visual vertical. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  The proprioceptive representation of eye position in monkey primary somatosensory cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  A. D. Van Beuzekom,et al.  Properties of the internal representation of gravity inferred from spatial-direction and body-tilt estimates. , 2000 .

[35]  Elizabeth T. Wilson,et al.  Mapping Proprioception across a 2D Horizontal Workspace , 2010, PloS one.

[36]  Lacquaniti,et al.  Visuo‐motor transformations for arm reaching , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control , 2022 .

[38]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Jean-René Duhamel,et al.  Optimal Sensorimotor Control in Eye Movement Sequences , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Multi-Sensory Weights Depend on Contextual Noise in Reference Frame Transformations , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  E. M. Klier,et al.  Human oculomotor system accounts for 3-D eye orientation in the visual-motor transformation for saccades. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Sensory transformations and the use of multiple reference frames for reach planning , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  J. J. Denier van der Gon,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information , 1999 .

[44]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Visuomotor velocity transformations for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  M. Goodale Transforming vision into action , 2011, Vision Research.