Perception of Emotions from Static Postures

Human synthetic characters in any computer simulated environment need to be capable of behaving the appropriate way of expressing emotion like normal human do. If the characters fail to express the suitable emotional expression, they will likely to break the users mood and belief. These imply that the key problem in designing synthetic characters is to make them believable in their overall behavior. Therefore the study was directed to construct human synthetic characters that can improve humans respond as well as identify intensity rating of emotions. Ekmans six emotional expressions [1], which are happiness, sadness, anger, surprise, fear and disgust were used because these basic emotions are clear, widely accepted and sufficient. An experiment was conducted to measure believable emotional expression from static images. Sets of virtual human static images of posed expressions of emotions were shown to the subjects. A total of 36 thirty-six volunteers (18 men and 18 women) took part in the experiment. The mean age was 29 years old. The subjects were asked to recognize the expressions by grouping them into six basic emotions and provide the level of emotion for each posture to study the intensity-rating tasks.

[1]  Mel Slater,et al.  Building Characters: Lessons Drawn from Virtual Environments , 2005 .

[2]  J. Cunningham,et al.  Children's decoding of emotion in expressive body movement: the development of cue attunement. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[3]  R. D. Walk,et al.  Perception of the smile and other emotions of the body and face at different distances , 1988 .

[4]  K. Scherer,et al.  Bodily expression of emotion , 2009 .

[5]  P. Ekman Emotion in the human face , 1982 .

[6]  M. D. Meijer The contribution of general features of body movement to the attribution of emotions , 1989 .

[7]  B. Gelder Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  Judith Greene,et al.  Learning to Use Statistical Tests in Psychology , 1982 .

[9]  K BurgoonJudee,et al.  Toward a more robust theory and measure of social presence , 2003 .

[10]  J. Montepare,et al.  The Use of Body Movements and Gestures as Cues to Emotions in Younger and Older Adults , 1999 .

[11]  W. S. Reilly,et al.  Believable Social and Emotional Agents. , 1996 .

[12]  Mel Slater,et al.  An Investigation of Presence Response across Variations in Visual Realism , 2004 .

[13]  M. Duke,et al.  Effects of Social Anxiety on Nonverbal Accuracy and Response Time I: Facial Expressions , 2004 .

[14]  W. Dittrich Action Categories and the Perception of Biological Motion , 1993, Perception.

[15]  L. Vistnes The Artist??s Complete Guide to Facial Expression , 1992 .

[16]  T. Dalgleish,et al.  Handbook of cognition and emotion , 1999 .

[17]  Frank Biocca,et al.  Toward a More Robust Theory and Measure of Social Presence: Review and Suggested Criteria , 2003, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[18]  S. Brownlow,et al.  Perception of movement and dancer characteristics from point-light displays of dance , 1997 .

[19]  G. Reeke The society of mind , 1991 .

[20]  Mark C. Coulson Attributing Emotion to Static Body Postures: Recognition Accuracy, Confusions, and Viewpoint Dependence , 2004 .

[21]  Boone Rt,et al.  Children's Decoding of Emotion in Expressive Body Movement: The Development of Cue Attunement. , 1998 .

[22]  Swann Pichon,et al.  Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[23]  P. Ekman An argument for basic emotions , 1992 .

[24]  Rosalind W. Picard Affective computing: challenges , 2003, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..