An Exploratory Study on the Relationship between Orientation Map Reading and Way-finding in Unfamiliar Environments

There are many ways to familiarize oneself with an unfamiliar environment, but most people use orientation maps to help them. In more complicated buildings, orientation maps may be posted everywhere, since they serve as important references for spatial cognition and wayfinding for users. A study on way-finding by Best (1970) identified a positive relationship between the number of choice points (hallway intersections) within a building and wayfinding difficulty. Weisman (1981) defined a number of environmental variables that people use to help orient themselves during way-finding, and categorized these variables into four classes: (1) signs, which provide directional information within a setting; (2) perceptual access, which provides a view to landmarks within or exterior to a building; (3) architectural differentiation, which is the ease with which different regions or landmarks within a building can be recognized; and (4) plan configuration, which is the configuration of a building's floor plan. Beaumont et al. (1984) interviewed building occupants and found that the layout of floor plans is equal in importance to other architectural features, such as the availability of signs, with respect to reported ease of way-finding. O'Neill (1991a) found that incremental increases in floor plan complexity reduce both the accuracy of one’s cognitive map and one’s wayfinding performance. O'Neill (1991b) also found that floor plan complexity reduces wayfinding performance, despite the presence of directional signage. Nichols et al. (1992) reported that the primary cause of way-finding difficulties in transportation centers is the complexity of possible paths. The accuracy of the information in one’s cognitive map can influence one’s way-finding performance (O'Neill, 1991a, 1991b). The cognitive map can store route and survey representations (Tolman, 1948), where route representations contain knowledge about individual places and the way in which they are connected through experience; in other words, the ‘travel-ability’ that exists between places (Kuipers, 1983). Montello (1991) proposed that the asymmetric structural design of streets is likely to mislead users: subjects tend to make more mistakes pointing out the position of an object and direction (east, west, south, and north) when on asymmetric streets than on intersecting ones. Passini (1999) argued that the acquisition of knowledge about places plays an important role in way-finding and determines one’s chance of finding one’s way successfully. If pedestrians acquire incorrect information about a place during way-finding, the chances are that they will get lost.

[1]  L Hamayon,et al.  Direction - Finding in Large Buildings , 1969 .

[2]  Ching-Yuan Lin,et al.  Exploratory research on reading cognition and escape-route planning using building evacuation plan diagrams. , 2008, Applied ergonomics.

[3]  Michael O'Neill,et al.  Evaluation of a Conceptual Model of Architectural Legibility , 1991 .

[4]  Cathryn Johns,et al.  Spatial learning: cognitive mapping in abstract virtual environments , 2003, AFRIGRAPH '03.

[5]  J R Wilson,et al.  Virtual environments applications and applied ergonomics. , 1999, Applied ergonomics.

[6]  Kay M. Stanney,et al.  Virtual environments , 2002 .

[7]  J. Jacko,et al.  The human-computer interaction handbook: fundamentals, evolving technologies and emerging applications , 2002 .

[8]  Rudy Darken Wayfinding in large-scale virtual worlds , 1995, CHI '95.

[9]  R. Golledge,et al.  Spatial Behavior: A Geographic Perspective , 1996 .

[10]  Kevin Lynch,et al.  The Image of the City , 1960 .

[11]  F Nichols,et al.  DESIGNING FOR PEDESTRIANS: A CAD-NETWORK ANALYSIS APPROACH , 1991 .

[12]  Hans-Otto Carmesin,et al.  A Taxonomy of Spatial Knowledge for Navigation and its Application to the Bremen Autonomous Wheelchair , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[13]  R. Golledge Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes , 2010 .

[14]  Benjamin Kuipers,et al.  The Cognitive Map: Could It Have Been Any Other Way? , 1983 .

[15]  Daniel R. Montello,et al.  Spatial Orientation and the Angularity of Urban Routes , 1991 .

[16]  Constantine Stephanidis,et al.  Virtual Prints: Leaving trails in Virtual Environments , 2002, EGVE.

[17]  R. Kitchin,et al.  Cognitive maps: What are they and why study them? , 1994 .

[18]  Rudy Darken,et al.  Wayfinding strategies and behaviors in large virtual worlds , 1996, CHI.

[19]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Using desktop virtual environments to investigate the role of landmarks , 2002, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[20]  Lawrence J. Najjar,et al.  An interactive environment for virtual manufacturing: the virtual work bench , 1999 .

[21]  E. Lindberg,et al.  Cognitive Mapping of Large-Scale Environments , 1984 .

[22]  Stephen C. Hirtle,et al.  The Nature of Landmarks for Real and Electronic Spaces , 1999, COSIT.

[23]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[24]  Kellogg S. Booth,et al.  Wayfinding in a Virtual Environment , 2000 .

[25]  Stephan Winter,et al.  Spatial Information Theory, 8th International Conference, COSIT 2007, Melbourne, Australia, September 19-23, 2007, Proceedings , 2007, COSIT.

[26]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  Nancy K. Innis Tolman, Edward C. , 2006 .

[28]  Jerry Weisman,et al.  Evaluating Architectural Legibility , 1981 .

[29]  Daniel R. Montello,et al.  Sex-Related Differences and Similarities in Geographic and Environmental Spatial Abilities , 1999 .

[30]  Tim Mullin,et al.  Virtual environment , 2006 .

[31]  T. Todd Elvins,et al.  VisFiles: virtually lost in virtual worlds—wayfinding without a cognitive map , 1997, COMG.

[32]  Leslie W. Trowbridge,et al.  Teaching Secondary School Science: Strategies for Developing Scientific Literacy , 1995 .

[33]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[34]  Paul U. Lee,et al.  Pictorial and Verbal Tools for Conveying Routes , 1999, COSIT.

[35]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[36]  Romedi Passini,et al.  Wayfinding in Architecture , 1984 .

[37]  Howard Rheingold,et al.  Virtual Reality , 1991 .

[38]  Michael O'Neill,et al.  Effects of Signage and Floor Plan Configuration on Wayfinding Accuracy , 1991 .

[39]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Spatial representation and behavior across the life span , 1981 .

[40]  J. Hudson A DIAMOND ANNIVERSARY , 1979 .

[41]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  HUMAN WAYFINDING AND COGNITIVE MAPS , 2003 .

[42]  John H. Bailey,et al.  Virtual spaces and real world places: transfer of route knowledge , 1996, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..