The effect of step size on straight-line orientation

Moving along a straight path is a surprisingly difficult task. This is because, with each ensuing step, noise is generated in the motor and sensory systems, causing the animal to deviate from its intended route. When relying solely on internal sensory information to correct for this noise, the directional error generated with each stride accumulates, ultimately leading to a curved path. In contrast, external compass cues effectively allow the animal to correct for errors in its bearing. Here, we studied straight-line orientation in two different sized dung beetles. This allowed us to characterize and model the size of the directional error generated with each step, in the absence of external visual compass cues (motor error) as well as in the presence of these cues (compass and motor errors). In addition, we model how dung beetles balance the influence of internal and external orientation cues as they orient along straight paths under the open sky. We conclude that the directional error that unavoidably accumulates as the beetle travels is inversely proportional to the step size of the insect, and that both beetle species weigh the two sources of directional information in a similar fashion.

[1]  SPARASSIDAE , 2020, Britain's Spiders.

[2]  M. Byrne,et al.  Straight-line orientation in the woodland-living beetle Sisyphus fasciculatus , 2019, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[3]  G. A. Pavlova The similarity of crawling mechanisms in aquatic and terrestrial gastropods , 2018, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[4]  Sönke Johnsen,et al.  The Earth’s Magnetic Field and Visual Landmarks Steer Migratory Flight Behavior in the Nocturnal Australian Bogong Moth , 2018, Current Biology.

[5]  A. Narendra,et al.  Miniaturisation decreases visual navigational competence in ants , 2018, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[6]  Barbara Webb,et al.  Visual Tracking of Small Animals in Cluttered Natural Environments Using a Freely Moving Camera , 2017, 2017 IEEE International Conference on Computer Vision Workshops (ICCVW).

[7]  William F. Towne,et al.  Honeybees use the skyline in orientation , 2017, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[8]  James J. Foster,et al.  Spectral information as an orientation cue in dung beetles , 2015, Biology Letters.

[9]  B. Webb,et al.  Optimal cue integration in ants , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[10]  Jochen Smolka,et al.  Neural coding underlying the cue preference for celestial orientation , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Jochen Smolka,et al.  Diurnal dung beetles use the intensity gradient and the polarization pattern of the sky for orientation , 2014, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[12]  R. Gegear,et al.  A magnetic compass aids monarch butterfly migration , 2014, Nature Communications.

[13]  Basil el Jundi,et al.  The role of the sun in the celestial compass of dung beetles , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  George V Lauder,et al.  Pectoral fins aid in navigation of a complex environment by bluegill sunfish under sensory deprivation conditions , 2013, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[15]  M. Byrne,et al.  Dung Beetles Use the Milky Way for Orientation , 2013, Current Biology.

[16]  Jochen Smolka,et al.  Dung beetles ignore landmarks for straight-line orientation , 2012, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[17]  Brett T. McClintock,et al.  A general discrete‐time modeling framework for animal movement using multistate random walks , 2012 .

[18]  M. Ernst,et al.  Walking Straight into Circles , 2009, Current Biology.

[19]  J. Downing,et al.  Fish movement and habitat use depends on water body size and shape , 2009 .

[20]  Allen Cheung,et al.  Animal navigation: general properties of directed walks , 2008, Biological Cybernetics.

[21]  Edward A. Codling,et al.  Random walk models in biology , 2008, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[22]  G. Dehnhardt,et al.  Harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) can steer by the stars , 2008, Animal Cognition.

[23]  R. Wehner,et al.  Vision in the nocturnal wandering spider Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae: Sparassidae) , 2008, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[24]  Allen Cheung,et al.  Animal navigation: the difficulty of moving in a straight line , 2007, Biological Cybernetics.

[25]  J. Rieser,et al.  Bayesian integration of spatial information. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  Sarah S. Greenleaf,et al.  Bee foraging ranges and their relationship to body size , 2007, Oecologia.

[27]  R. Wehner,et al.  Wind and sky as compass cues in desert ant navigation , 2007, Naturwissenschaften.

[28]  Rüdiger Wehner,et al.  The Ant Odometer: Stepping on Stilts and Stumps , 2006, Science.

[29]  M. Boyce,et al.  WOLVES INFLUENCE ELK MOVEMENTS: BEHAVIOR SHAPES A TROPHIC CASCADE IN YELLOWSTONE NATIONAL PARK , 2005 .

[30]  W. Cochran,et al.  Migrating Songbirds Recalibrate Their Magnetic Compass Daily from Twilight Cues , 2004, Science.

[31]  Eric J. Warrant,et al.  Lunar orientation in a beetle , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[32]  Eric J. Warrant,et al.  Visual cues used by ball-rolling dung beetles for orientation , 2003, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[33]  R Wehner,et al.  Egocentric information helps desert ants to navigate around familiar obstacles. , 2001, The Journal of experimental biology.

[34]  Elizabeth E. Crone,et al.  EDGE-MEDIATED DISPERSAL BEHAVIOR IN A PRAIRIE BUTTERFLY , 2001 .

[35]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Interactions between idiothetic cues and external landmarks in the control of place cells and head direction cells. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  P. Thompson,et al.  The influence of body size and sex on the characteristics of harbour seal foraging trips , 1998 .

[37]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Piloting and dead reckoning dissociated by fimbria-fornix lesions in a rat food carrying task , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[38]  J. Phillips,et al.  Magnetic compass orientation is eliminated under near-infrared light in the eastern red-spotted newt Notophthalmus viridescens , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[39]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Distinguishing between elementary orientation mechanisms by means of path analysis , 1992, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  B. Heinrich,et al.  Roles of Endothermy and Size in Inter- and Intraspecific Competition for Elephant Dung in an African Dung Beetle, Scarabaeus laevistriatus , 1979, Physiological Zoology.

[41]  A. Harestad,et al.  Home Range and Body Weight‐‐A Reevaluation , 1979 .

[42]  Friedrich G. Barth,et al.  Slit sense organs and kinesthetic orientation , 1971, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[43]  Edward A Codling,et al.  Navigational efficiency in a biased and correlated random walk model of individual animal movement. , 2018, Ecology.

[44]  A. Ugolini,et al.  Equatorial sandhoppers use body scans to detect the earth’s magnetic field , 2005, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[45]  R. Wehner,et al.  Visual navigation in insects: coupling of egocentric and geocentric information , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[46]  A S Etienne,et al.  Path integration in mammals and its interaction with visual landmarks. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[47]  E. Batschelet Circular statistics in biology , 1981 .