Interpretation of informational questions modulated by joint knowledge and intonational contours

Abstract We examine processes by which dialogue partners form and use representations of joint knowledge, or common ground, during on-line language processing. Eye-tracked participants interpreted wh-questions that inquired about task-relevant objects during interactive conversation. Some objects were known to both speaker and listener, and thus in common ground, whereas others were only known to the listener, and thus in privileged ground. Questions were produced with a typical, falling intonation (Experiment 1) or with either falling or rising intonation (Experiments 2–3). Unlike the falling contour, the rising contour can indicate a request for clarification about previously mentioned information. Participants interpreted falling-contour questions as asking about privileged-ground objects. By contrast, rising questions elicited more consideration of common-ground objects. Directly comparing questions that were produced during live conversation vs. questions that were pre-recorded revealed that this sensitivity to common vs. privileged ground emerged only during live conversation. Finally, individual difference analyses in all three experiments did not support the claim that individuals fail to take perspective when executive function is limited. Taken together, these findings provide evidence for the on-line integration of perspective and intonation during conversational language processing. The lack of perspective effects in non-interactive settings speaks to the inherently interactive nature of conversational processes.

[1]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Christine A. Gunlogson True to Form: Rising and Falling Declaratives as Questions in English , 2003 .

[3]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  The Influence of Partner-Specific Memory Associations on Picture Naming: A Failure to Replicate Horton (2007) , 2014, PloS one.

[4]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Coordinating beliefs in conversation , 1992 .

[5]  S. Brennan,et al.  When conceptual pacts are broken: Partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions , 2003 .

[6]  Anna K. Kuhlen,et al.  Language in Dialogue: When Confederates Might Be Hazardous to Your Data , 2022 .

[7]  Ting Qian,et al.  Rapid Expectation Adaptation during Syntactic Comprehension , 2013, PloS one.

[8]  N. Epley,et al.  In the mood to get over yourself: mood affects theory-of-mind use. , 2008, Emotion.

[9]  Sarah Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Talking in another person's shoes: Incremental perspective-taking in language processing , 2011, Dialogue Discourse.

[10]  H. H. Clark,et al.  References in Conversation Between Experts and Novices , 1987 .

[11]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation , 2003 .

[12]  Rachel Ryskin,et al.  Visuospatial perspective-taking in conversation and the role of bilingual experience , 2014 .

[13]  D. Barr,et al.  Taking Perspective in Conversation: The Role of Mutual Knowledge in Comprehension , 2000, Psychological science.

[14]  W. Horton,et al.  Anticipating who will say what: The influence of speaker-specific memory associations on reference resolution , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[15]  Elizabeth S. Nilsen,et al.  The relations between children’s communicative perspective-taking and executive functioning , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  S. Brennan,et al.  Addressees' needs influence speakers' early syntactic choices , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding , 2004 .

[19]  Sarah Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Addressees distinguish shared from private information when interpreting questions during interactive conversation , 2008, Cognition.

[20]  B. Keysar,et al.  You said it before and you'll say it again: expectations of consistency in communication. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  Accent and reference resolution in spoken-language comprehension , 2002 .

[22]  Kristen S. Gorman,et al.  What's learned together stays together: speakers' choice of referring expression reflects shared experience. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  Duane G. Watson,et al.  Experimental and theoretical advances in prosody: A review , 2010, Language and cognitive processes.

[24]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Contributing to Discourse , 1989, Cogn. Sci..

[25]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[26]  Nick C Fox,et al.  Gene-Wide Analysis Detects Two New Susceptibility Genes for Alzheimer's Disease , 2014, PLoS ONE.

[27]  William S Horton,et al.  The influence of partner-specific memory associations on language production: Evidence from picture naming , 2007, Language and cognitive processes.

[28]  D. Barr,et al.  Limits on theory of mind use in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[29]  N. Epley,et al.  Reflexively mindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention , 2010 .

[30]  D. Barr Pragmatic expectations and linguistic evidence: Listeners anticipate but do not integrate common ground , 2008, Cognition.

[31]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Understanding by addressees and overhearers , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[33]  C. Bartels,et al.  Towards a compositional interpretation of English statement and question intonation , 1997 .

[34]  D. Barr,et al.  Perspective-free pragmatics: Broken precedents and the recovery-from-preemption hypothesis , 2007 .

[35]  S. Brennan,et al.  Computer-Mediated Communication: Cognitive Science Approach , 2006 .

[36]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Beyond common and privileged: Gradient representations of common ground in real-time language use , 2012 .

[37]  Catherine Marshall,et al.  Reference Diaries , 1978, TINLAP.

[38]  Timothy M. Gann,et al.  Anticipatory baseline effects and information integration in visual world studies. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[39]  D. Barr,et al.  The role of fillers in listener attributions for speaker disfluency , 2010 .

[40]  A. Sanford,et al.  Expectations in counterfactual and theory of mind reasoning , 2010 .

[41]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[42]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[43]  D. Barr,et al.  Anchoring Comprehension in Linguistic Precedents , 2002 .

[44]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[45]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  The role of perspective in identifying domains of reference , 2008, Cognition.

[46]  H. H. Clark Arenas of language use , 1993 .

[47]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Grounding in communication , 1991, Perspectives on socially shared cognition.

[48]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Referring as a collaborative process , 1986, Cognition.

[49]  Duane G. Watson,et al.  Recognition memory reveals just how CONTRASTIVE contrastive accenting really is. , 2010, Journal of memory and language.

[50]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Do priming effects in dialogue reflect partner- or task-based expectations? , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[51]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[52]  Mari Ostendorf,et al.  TOBI: a standard for labeling English prosody , 1992, ICSLP.

[53]  Liane Wardlow Individual differences in speakers’ perspective taking: The roles of executive control and working memory , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[54]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. , 2009, Journal of memory and language.