Modeling the Dynamics of Evaluation

We present a neural network implementation of central components of the iterative reprocessing (IR) model. The IR model argues that the evaluation of social stimuli (attitudes, stereotypes) is the result of the IR of stimuli in a hierarchy of neural systems: The evaluation of social stimuli develops and changes over processing. The network has a multilevel, bidirectional feedback evaluation system that integrates initial perceptual processing and later developing semantic processing. The network processes stimuli (e.g., an individual’s appearance) over repeated iterations, with increasingly higher levels of semantic processing over time. As a result, the network’s evaluations of stimuli evolve. We discuss the implications of the network for a number of different issues involved in attitudes and social evaluation. The success of the network supports the IR model framework and provides new insights into attitude theory.

[1]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Evaluation is a dynamic process: Moving beyond dual system models. , 2012 .

[2]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience: Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain , 2000 .

[3]  T. Ito,et al.  Tracking the dynamics of the social brain: ERP approaches for social cognitive and affective neuroscience. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[4]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Agent-Based Modeling: A New Approach for Theory Building in Social Psychology , 2007, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[5]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Neural correlates of automatic beliefs about gender and race , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[6]  Joshua Correll,et al.  Dangerous Enough: Moderating Racial Bias with Contextual Threat Cues. , 2011, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[7]  Brian Mingus,et al.  The Emergent neural modeling system , 2008, Neural Networks.

[8]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Are associative and propositional processes qualitatively distinct? Comment on Gawronski and Bodenhausen (2006). , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  B. T. Evans Jonathan,et al.  Dual-process theories , 2017 .

[10]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Attitude Representation: Attitudes as Patterns in a Distributed, Connectionist Representational System , 2007 .

[11]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  12. Formation, change, and contextualization of mental associations: determinants and principles of variations in implicit measures , 2010 .

[12]  Weixuan Yang,et al.  Design and Analysis of Nanotube-Based Memory Cells , 2008, Nanoscale Research Letters.

[13]  Rick Dale,et al.  The Self-Organization of Explicit Attitudes , 2009, Psychological science.

[14]  I. Blair,et al.  The Malleability of Automatic Stereotypes and Prejudice , 2002 .

[15]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Performance on Indirect Measures of Race Evaluation Predicts Amygdala Activation , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.3 Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective , 2022 .

[17]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. , 1994 .

[18]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  The iterative reprocessing model: A multilevel framework for attitudes and evaluation. , 2007 .

[19]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  The Flexible Correction Model: The Role of Naive Theories of Bias in Bias Correction , 1997 .

[20]  Jonathan B Freeman,et al.  MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[21]  J. N. Bassili,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Attitudes: Research, Challenges, and Theory. , 2005 .

[22]  J. V. Bavel,et al.  Social identity shapes social perception and evaluation: Using neuroimaging to look inside the social brain , 2013 .

[23]  C. Judd,et al.  Spontaneous prejudice in context: variability in automatically activated attitudes. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Induction of Multiscale Temporal Structure , 1991, NIPS.

[25]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  G. Bodenhausen,et al.  Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[27]  A. Hall,et al.  Adaptive Switching Circuits , 2016 .

[28]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  A model of dual attitudes. , 2000, Psychological review.

[29]  D. Carlston The Oxford handbook of social cognition , 2013 .

[30]  Russell H. Fazio,et al.  Attitudes as object-evaluation associations: Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude accessibility. , 1995 .

[31]  M. Brewer,et al.  Dual processes in the cognitive representation of persons and social categories. , 1999 .

[32]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Partitioning the domain of social inference: dual mode and systems models and their alternatives. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[33]  Richard E Petty,et al.  Contextual moderation of racial bias: the impact of social roles on controlled and automatically activated attitudes. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  S. Read,et al.  A general connectionist model of attitude structure and change: the ACS (Attitudes as Constraint Satisfaction) model. , 2008, Psychological review.

[35]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[36]  Jonathan Evans Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[37]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[38]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Intuitive and Deliberate Judgments Are Based on Common Principles This Article Has Been Corrected. See Last Page , 2022 .

[39]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Rapid social perception is flexible: approach and avoidance motivational states shape P100 responses to other-race faces , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[41]  P. Thagard,et al.  FORMING IMPRESSIONS FROM STEREOTYPES, TRAITS, AND BEHAVIORS : A PARALLEL-CONSTRAINT-SATISFACTION THEORY , 1996 .

[42]  N. Ambady,et al.  A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. , 2011, Psychological review.

[43]  René J. Huster,et al.  Methods for Simultaneous EEG-fMRI: An Introductory Review , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Beyond Bipolar Conceptualizations and Measures: The Case of Attitudes and Evaluative Space , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[45]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Implicit measures: A normative analysis and review. , 2009, Psychological bulletin.

[46]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Reactions to a black professional: motivated inhibition and activation of conflicting stereotypes. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  Jonathan Evans In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[48]  R. Fazio Attitudes as Object-Evaluation Associations of Varying Strength. , 2007, Social cognition.

[49]  G. Keren,et al.  Two Is Not Always Better Than One , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[50]  T. Ito,et al.  Race and gender on the brain: electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  Thomas A. Farmer,et al.  Hand in Motion Reveals Mind in Motion , 2011, Front. Psychology.