Automatic motor activation by mere instruction

Previous behavioral studies have shown that instructions about stimulus–response (S-R) mappings can influence task performance even when these instructions are irrelevant for the current task. In the present study, we tested whether automatic effects of S–R instructions occur because the instructed stimuli automatically activate their corresponding responses. We registered the lateralized readiness potentials (LRPs) that were evoked by the instructed stimuli while participants were performing a task for which those mappings were irrelevant. Instructed S–R mappings clearly affected task performance in electrophysiological and behavioral measures. The LRP was found to deflect in the direction of the response tendency that corresponded with the instructed S–R mapping. Early activation of the instructed response was observed but occurred predominantly on slow trials. In contrast, response conflict evoked by instructed S–R mappings did not modulate the N2 amplitude. The results strongly suggest that, like experienced S–R mappings, instructed S–R mappings can lead to automatic response activation, but possibly via a different route.

[1]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[2]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  The representation of instructions in working memory leads to autonomous response activation: Evidence from the first trials in the flanker paradigm , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  J T Mordkoff,et al.  Detecting the onset of the lateralized readiness potential: a comparison of available methods and procedures. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[5]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  A Model-Averaging Approach to Replication : The Case of p rep , 2009 .

[6]  R. Ratcliff Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  WordGen: A tool for word selection and nonword generation in Dutch, English, German, and French , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[8]  R. Gaschler,et al.  Instruction-induced feature binding , 2007, Psychological research.

[9]  J. Houwer,et al.  Instruction-based response activation depends on task preparation , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  W. Sommer,et al.  Are fingers special? Evidence about movement preparation from event-related brain potentials. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[11]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[12]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Multidimensional vector model of stimulus-response compatibility. , 2012, Psychological review.

[13]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[14]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Hannes Ruge,et al.  Rapid formation of pragmatic rule representations in the human brain during instruction-based learning. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[17]  W. Schwarz,et al.  Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[18]  Martin Eimer,et al.  The lateralized readiness potential as an on-line measure of central response activation processes , 1998 .

[19]  Hiroaki Masaki,et al.  The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[20]  Luca Citi,et al.  Reaction-time binning: a simple method for increasing the resolving power of ERP averages. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[21]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Using the jackknife-based scoring method for measuring LRP onset effects in factorial designs. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[23]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  The representation of instructions operates like a prepared reflex: flanker compatibility effects found in first trial following S-R instructions. , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[24]  H. Vaughan,et al.  Cortical potentials associated with voluntary movements in the monkey , 1975, Brain Research.

[25]  Marcel Brass,et al.  The implementation of verbal instructions: Dissociating motor preparation from the formation of stimulus–response associations , 2012, NeuroImage.

[26]  M. Brass,et al.  The implementation of verbal instructions: An fMRI study , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[27]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[28]  Stefaan Vandorpe,et al.  Further evidence for the role of mode-independent short-term associations in spatial Simon effects , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  M. Muggeo,et al.  segmented: An R package to Fit Regression Models with Broken-Line Relationships , 2008 .

[30]  M. W. Molen,et al.  Temporal dynamics of interference in Simon and Eriksen tasks considered within the context of a dual-process model , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Neural Activation During Response Competition , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  M. Coles Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[33]  H. Leuthold,et al.  Automatic response activation in sequential affective priming: an ERP study. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[34]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. , 2011 .

[35]  A. Cools,et al.  Dipole source analysis suggests selective modulation of the supplementary motor area contribution to the readiness potential. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[36]  F. Smulders Simplifying jackknifing of ERPs and getting more out of it: retrieving estimates of participants' latencies. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[37]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[38]  Cheryl L. Dickter,et al.  Strategic control and medial frontal negativity: beyond errors and response conflict. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[39]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  When planning results in loss of control: intention-based reflexivity and working-memory , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  Hisae Gemba,et al.  Potential related to no-go reaction in go/no-go hand movement with discrimination between tone stimuli of different frequencies in the monkey , 1990, Brain Research.

[41]  Randall C. O'Reilly,et al.  Assembling Old Tricks for New Tasks: A Neural Model of Instructional Learning and Control , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[43]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[44]  Nachshon Meiran,et al.  Working memory load but not multitasking eliminates the prepared reflex: further evidence from the adapted flanker paradigm. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[45]  J. de Houwer,et al.  Instruction-based task-rule congruency effects. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[47]  M Eimer,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: evidence from psychophysiological studies. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  W. Sommer,et al.  Partial advance information and response preparation: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  T. Okita,et al.  An ERP investigation of task switching using a flanker paradigm , 2010, Brain Research.

[50]  Jeff Miller,et al.  The Lateralized Readiness Potential , 2011 .

[51]  S. Luck An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique , 2005 .

[52]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Novelty and conflict in the categorization of complex stimuli. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[53]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[54]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Tscope: A C library for programming cognitive experiments on the MS Windows platform , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[55]  L Kaufman,et al.  Magnetic field of the human sensorimotor cortex. , 1982, The International journal of neuroscience.

[56]  Dorit Wenke,et al.  Strategic influences on implementing instructions for future actions , 2009, Psychological research.

[57]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[58]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[59]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Neural Correlates of Overcoming Interference from Instructed and Implemented Stimulus–Response Associations , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[61]  Wim Fias,et al.  Further evidence that the SNARC effect is processed along a dual-route architecture: Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2006, Experimental psychology.

[62]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  A new method for the estimation of the onset of the lateralized readiness potential (LRP) , 1998 .

[63]  T. Verguts,et al.  Word and deed: A computational model of instruction following , 2012, Brain Research.

[64]  Werner Sommer,et al.  Separating stimulus-driven and response-related LRP components with Residue Iteration Decomposition (RIDE). , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[65]  Yoav Kessler,et al.  The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.