Domain-specificity in the acquisition of non-adjacent dependencies

At the forefront of investigations into the cognitive underpinnings of language acquisition is the question of domain-specificity, i.e. whether the processes involved in learning language are unique to language. Recent investigations suggest that the mechanisms employed in language learning are also involved in sequential learning of non-linguistic stimuli and are therefore domain-general. Non-adjacent dependencies are an important feature of natural languages. They describe relationships between two elements separated by an arbitrary number of intervening items, and thus potentially pose a challenge for learners. As a hallmark of natural languages they are ubiquitous, an example from English being subject-verb agreement: The socks on the floor are red. Here, learners are required to track the dependencies amongst the two underlined elements across an intervening prepositional phrase. Importantly, it has been shown that non-adjacent dependencies can be learned in the linguistic (Gomez, 2002) and non-linguistic (Creel, Newport & Aslin, 2004) domain. The majority of work presented in this thesis is based on Gomez’s (2002) artificial language learning experiment involving non-adjacent dependencies, adapted to directly compare adults’ learning in the linguistic and non-linguistic domain, in order to build a comprehensive map showing factors and conditions that enhance/ inhibit the learnability of non-adjacencies. Experiment 1 shows that the Gestalt Principle of Similarity is not a requirement for the detection of non-adjacent dependencies in the linguistic domain. Experiment 2 aims to explore the robustness of the ability to track non-adjacent regularities between linguistic elements by removing cues that indicate the correct level of analysis (i.e. interword breaks). Experiments 3 and 4 study domain-specificity in the acquisition of non-adjacencies, and show that non-adjacent dependencies are learnable in the linguistic and nonlinguistic domain, provided that the non-linguistic materials are simple and lacking internal structure. However, language is rich in internal structure: it is combinatorial on the phonemic/ orthographic level in that it recombines elements (phonemes/graphemes) to form larger units. When exposed to non-linguistic stimuli which capture this componential character of language, adult participants fail to detect the non-adjacencies. However, when exposed to non-componential non-linguistic materials, adult participants succeed in learning the non-adjacent dependencies. Experiment 5 looks at modality effects in the acquisition of non-adjacent dependencies across the linguistic and non-linguistic domain. Experiment 6 provides evidence that high familiarity with componential non-linguistic patterns does not result in the correct extraction of non-adjacencies in sequence learning tasks involving these patterns. Overall, the work presented here demonstrates that the acquisition of nonadjacent dependencies is a domain-general ability, which is guided by stimulus simplicity.

[1]  Mark Aronoff,et al.  Contemporary linguistics: An introduction , 1989 .

[2]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Complex Systems in Language Evolution: the Cultural Emergence of Compositional Structure , 2003, Adv. Complex Syst..

[3]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  The learnability of abstract syntactic principles , 2011, Cognition.

[4]  R. Gómez Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.

[5]  Barbara C. Scholz,et al.  Empirical assessment of stimulus poverty arguments , 2002 .

[6]  J. Henderson,et al.  Object-based attentional selection in scene viewing. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[7]  B. Zellner Pauses and the Temporal Structure of Speech 3 , 1994 .

[8]  Nick Chater,et al.  Reduction of Uncertainty in Human Sequential Learning: Evidence from Artificial Grammar Learning , 2003 .

[9]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Phonotactic and Prosodic Effects on Word Segmentation in Infants , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  Mathias Bartels,et al.  Pausing for thought: engagement of left temporal cortex during pauses in speech , 2004, NeuroImage.

[11]  Sarah C. Creel,et al.  Distant melodies: statistical learning of nonadjacent dependencies in tone sequences. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  A. Decasper,et al.  Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds , 1986 .

[13]  E. Newport,et al.  WORD SEGMENTATION : THE ROLE OF DISTRIBUTIONAL CUES , 1996 .

[14]  K. Okanoya,et al.  Statistical and Prosodic Cues for Song Segmentation Learning by Bengalese Finches (Lonchura striata var. domestica) , 2010 .

[15]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Phonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants , 2001, Cognition.

[17]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  The development of intersensory temporal perception: an epigenetic systems/limitations view. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  Gabriel J. L. Beckers,et al.  Zebra finches exhibit speaker-independent phonetic perception of human speech , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  Marina Nespor,et al.  Signal-Driven Computations in Speech Processing , 2002, Science.

[20]  E. Spelke,et al.  Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change , 1994 .

[21]  Michael A. Stadler,et al.  Handbook of implicit learning , 1998 .

[22]  A. Friederici Processing local transitions versus long-distance syntactic hierarchies , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Neocortex Size, Group Size, and the Evolution of Language , 1993, Current Anthropology.

[24]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[25]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Three models for the description of language , 1956, IRE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[26]  D. Donaldson,et al.  Listening to the sound of silence: Investigating the consequences of disfluent silent pauses in speech for listeners , 2010 .

[27]  Nick Chater,et al.  Variability is the spice of learning, and a crucial ingredient for detecting and generalizing in nonadjacent dependencies , 2004 .

[28]  J. Weissenborn,et al.  The Recognition of Discontinuous Verbal Dependencies by German 19-Month-Olds: Evidence for Lexical and Structural Influences on Children's Early Processing Capacities , 2006 .

[29]  F. Pulvermüller Brain embodiment of syntax and grammar: Discrete combinatorial mechanisms spelt out in neuronal circuits , 2010, Brain and Language.

[30]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  The power of statistical learning: No need for algebraic rules , 2020, Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[31]  M. Wertheimer,et al.  A source book of Gestalt psychology. , 1939 .

[32]  Robert C. Mathews,et al.  Abstractness of implicit knowledge: A cognitive evolutionary perspective. , 1997 .

[33]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Implicit learning of nonlocal musical rules: implicitly learning more than chunks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  M. Hauser,et al.  Learning at a distance II. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies in a non-human primate , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[36]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002, Science.

[37]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[38]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[39]  W. Fitch,et al.  Computational Constraints on Syntactic Processing in a Nonhuman Primate , 2004, Science.

[40]  A. Rey,et al.  Does the mastery of center-embedded linguistic structures distinguish humans from nonhuman primates? , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[41]  Andrew Radford,et al.  Syntactic Theory and the Structure of English: A Minimalist Approach , 1997 .

[42]  J. Fodor,et al.  The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology , 1984 .

[43]  Karsten Steinhauer,et al.  Brain signatures of artificial language processing: Evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[45]  Pienie Zwitserlood,et al.  Syntactic structure and artificial grammar learning: The learnability of embedded hierarchical structures , 2008, Cognition.

[46]  Richard J. Tunney,et al.  The transfer effect in artificial grammar learning: Reappraising the evidence on the transfer of sequential dependencies. , 1999 .

[47]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Language and problems of knowledge : the Managua lectures , 1990 .

[48]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Psychology and language : an introduction to psycholinguistics , 1979 .

[49]  M. Corballis From Hand to Mouth: The Origins of Language , 2002 .

[50]  J. Saffran Constraints on Statistical Language Learning , 2002 .

[51]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[52]  Mayumi Adachi,et al.  Expectancy in melody: tests of children and adults. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  E. Newport,et al.  Learning at a distance I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies , 2004, Cognitive Psychology.

[54]  L. Holt,et al.  Incidental categorization of spectrally complex non-invariant auditory stimuli in a computer game task. , 2005, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[55]  Massimo Piattelli-Palmarini,et al.  Language and Learning: The Debate Between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky , 1980 .

[56]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[57]  Peter W. Jusczyk,et al.  How infants begin to extract words from speech , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[58]  Thomas Goschke,et al.  Implicit learning of perceptual and motor sequences: Evidence for independent learning systems. , 1998 .

[59]  S. Pinker,et al.  Natural language and natural selection , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[60]  Colin M. Brown,et al.  The syntactic positive shift (sps) as an erp measure of syntactic processing , 1993 .

[61]  L. Cosmides,et al.  The psychological foundations of culture. , 1992 .

[62]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Seeing and Hearing in Space and Time: Effects of Modality and Presentation Rate on Implicit Statistical Learning , 2022 .

[63]  J. Prinz Is the mind really modular , 2005 .

[64]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Phonological typicality influences on-line sentence comprehension , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[65]  R. Gómez,et al.  Infant artificial language learning and language acquisition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  J. Saffran,et al.  Dog is a dog is a dog: Infant rule learning is not specific to language , 2007, Cognition.

[67]  Christopher M. Conway,et al.  Neurocognitive Basis of Implicit Learning of Sequential Structure and Its Relation to Language Processing , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[68]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  The basis of transfer in artificial grammar learning , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[69]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[70]  R. Stainton Contemporary debates in cognitive science , 2006 .

[71]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  The E-Z Reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models , 2003, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[72]  M. Wheeler,et al.  The Grain of Domains: The Evolutionary-Psychological Case Against Domain-General Cognition , 2004 .

[73]  R. Gómez,et al.  Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge , 1999, Cognition.

[74]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Information from multiple modalities helps 5-month-olds learn abstract rules. , 2009, Developmental science.

[75]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[76]  Robin I. M. Dunbar The Origin and Subsequent Evolution of Language , 2003 .

[77]  Kazuo Okanoya,et al.  Visual statistical learning of shape sequences: An ERP study , 2009, Neuroscience Research.

[78]  M. Wertheimer,et al.  Gestalt Theory , 2019, Theories and Applications of Counseling and Psychotherapy: Relevance Across Cultures and Settings.

[79]  A. Friederici,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for Two Steps in Syntactic Analysis: Early Automatic and Late Controlled Processes , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  L. Cosmides,et al.  The Adapted mind : evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture , 1992 .

[81]  J. Henderson,et al.  Accurate visual memory for previously attended objects in natural scenes , 2002 .

[82]  Nick Chater,et al.  Phonology impacts segmentation and generalization in speech processing , 2005 .

[83]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Language as shaped by the brain. , 2008, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[84]  A. Anwander,et al.  The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional localization and structural connectivity , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[85]  Jutta L. Mueller,et al.  The Role of Pause Cues in Language Learning: The Emergence of Event-related Potentials Related to Sequence Processing , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[86]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Location, location, location: development of spatiotemporal sequence learning in infancy. , 2007, Child development.

[87]  A. Reber Implicit learning of artificial grammars , 1967 .

[88]  Tobias Johansson,et al.  Strengthening the case for stimulus-specificity in artificial grammar learning: no evidence for abstract representations with extended exposure. , 2009, Experimental psychology.

[89]  Richard N Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of adjacent and nonadjacent dependencies among nonlinguistic sounds , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[90]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[91]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  The case for massively modular models of mind , 2004 .

[92]  R. Aslin,et al.  Statistical learning of higher-order temporal structure from visual shape sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[93]  A. Friederici,et al.  Event-related brain potentials during natural speech processing: effects of semantic, morphological and syntactic violations. , 1993, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[94]  Ellen F. Lau,et al.  The role of structural prediction in rapid syntactic analysis , 2006, Brain and Language.

[95]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.

[96]  S. Liversedge,et al.  Saccadic eye movements and cognition , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[97]  A. Cutler,et al.  Vowel harmony and speech segmentation in Finnish , 1997 .

[98]  A. Harcourt The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Patterns of Behavior, Jane Goodall. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachussets (1986), xii, +671. Price $30 , 1988 .

[99]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[100]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .

[101]  Paul C. Quinn,et al.  Part—whole perception in early infancy: Evidence for perceptual grouping produced by lightness similarity , 1993 .

[102]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Abstract Rule Learning for Visual Sequences in 8- and 11-Month-Olds. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[103]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Sensitivity to discontinuous dependencies in language learners: evidence for limitations in processing space , 1998, Cognition.

[104]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Evidence for auditory temporal distinctiveness: modality effects in order and frequency judgments. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[105]  C. Philip Beaman,et al.  Inverting the modality effect in serial recall , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[106]  John J. Ohala,et al.  Towards a universal, phonetically-based, theory of vowel harmony , 1994, ICSLP.

[107]  A. Decasper,et al.  Prenatal experience with low-frequency maternal-voice sounds influence neonatal perception of maternal voice samples , 1987 .

[108]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Word Segmentation by 8-Month-Olds: When Speech Cues Count More Than Statistics , 2001 .

[109]  Lori L. Holt,et al.  Expertise with Artificial Nonspeech Sounds Recruits Speech-Sensitive Cortical Regions , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[110]  S. Levinson,et al.  The myth of language universals: language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. , 2009, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[111]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Iterated Learning: A Framework for the Emergence of Language , 2003, Artificial Life.

[112]  M. Hauser,et al.  Segmentation of the speech stream in a non-human primate: statistical learning in cotton-top tamarins , 2001, Cognition.

[113]  Mary R. Newsome,et al.  The Beginnings of Word Segmentation in English-Learning Infants , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[114]  J. Henderson,et al.  High-level scene perception. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[115]  Nick Chater,et al.  Phonology impacts segmentation in online speech processing , 2005 .

[116]  R. Gómez,et al.  The Developmental Trajectory of Nonadjacent Dependency Learning. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[117]  J. Henderson Regarding Scenes , 2007 .

[118]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  On-Line Individual Differences in Statistical Learning Predict Language Processing , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[119]  Robert Lickliter,et al.  Increasing task difficulty enhances effects of intersensory redundancy: testing a new prediction of the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis. , 2010, Developmental science.

[120]  C. F. Hockett The origin of speech. , 1960, Scientific American.

[121]  Mapping across Domains Without Feedback: A Neural Network Model of Transfer of Implicit Knowledge , 1999 .

[122]  Geraint A. Wiggins,et al.  EXPECTATION IN MELODY: THE INFLUENCE OF CONTEXT AND LEARNING , 2006 .

[123]  M. Wertheimer Laws of organization in perceptual forms. , 1938 .

[124]  D J Field,et al.  Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[125]  Lori L. Holt,et al.  Incidental categorization of spectrally complex non-invariant auditory stimuli in a computer game task , 2005 .

[126]  Muhammad Ali Khalidi,et al.  Innateness and Domain Specificity , 2001 .

[127]  B. Scholl,et al.  The Automaticity of Visual Statistical Learning Statistical Learning , 2005 .

[128]  L. Cosmides,et al.  Mapping the mind: Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization , 1994 .

[129]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Infant Rule Learning Facilitated by Speech , 2007, Psychological science.