Intermittent visual information and the multiple time scales of visual motor control of continuous isometric force production

In an experiment, we examined the effect of intermittency (from 25.6 Hz to 0.2 Hz) of visual information on continuous isometric force production as a function of force level (5%, 10%, 25%, and 50% of maximal voluntary contraction [MVC]). The amount of force variability decreased and the irregularity of force output increased as a function of increased visual intermittency rate. Vision was found to have an influence on the frequency structure of force output up to 12 Hz, and the 25% MVC force level had more high-frequency modulations with higher rates of visual information. The effective use of intermittent visual information is mediated nonlinearly by force level, and there are multiple time scales of visual control (range, ∼0-12 Hz) that are postulated to be a function of both feedback and feedforward control processes.

[1]  G. Fullerton Psychology and physiology. , 1896 .

[2]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[3]  M A VINCE,et al.  Corrective Movements in a Pursuit Task 1 , 1948, Quarterly journal of experimental physiology and cognate medical sciences.

[4]  J. Deutsch Perception and Communication , 1958, Nature.

[5]  J. A. Adams,et al.  Short-term memory for motor responses. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  M. Posner,et al.  Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  J. Thomson How do we use visual information to control locomotion? , 1980, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  W. M. Smith,et al.  The effects of delayed and displaced visual feedback on motor control. , 1980, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Claes von Hofsten,et al.  Dialogue on perception and action , 1982 .

[11]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: theoretical distinctions and empirical results. , 1988, Acta psychologica.

[14]  A. Sanders Issues and trends in the debate on discrete vs. continuous processing of information , 1990 .

[15]  D. Elliott Intermittent visual pickup and goal directed movement: a review , 1990 .

[16]  S M Pincus,et al.  Approximate entropy as a measure of system complexity. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .

[18]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Chapter 1 Visual Processing Time and the Control of Movement , 1992 .

[19]  D. Elliott Chapter 2 Intermittent Versus Continuous Control of Manual Aiming Movements , 1992 .

[20]  R. Miall,et al.  Intermittency in human manual tracking tasks. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  H. Freund,et al.  The role of basal ganglia in rhythmic movement. , 1993, Advances in neurology.

[22]  A. Goldberger,et al.  Finite-size effects on long-range correlations: implications for analyzing DNA sequences. , 1993, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[23]  D L Gilden,et al.  1/f noise in human cognition. , 1995, Science.

[24]  G. P. van Galen,et al.  How continuous are discrete notions of human information processing , 1995 .

[25]  L. Lipsitz Age-related changes in the "complexity" of cardiovascular dynamics: A potential marker of vulnerability to disease. , 1995, Chaos.

[26]  Jeffrey M. Hausdorff,et al.  Altered fractal dynamics of gait: reduced stride-interval correlations with aging and Huntington's disease. , 1997, Journal of applied physiology.

[27]  K. Newell,et al.  Noise, information transmission, and force variability. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  K. Newell,et al.  Intermittency in the control of continuous force production. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  K M Newell,et al.  Variability and Noise in Continuous Force Production , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  K. Newell,et al.  Age differences in noise and variability of isometric force production. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[32]  Lawrence M. Ward,et al.  Dynamical Cognitive Science , 2001 .

[33]  K. Newell,et al.  Effects of aging on force variability, single motor unit discharge patterns, and the structure of 10, 20, and 40 Hz EMG activity , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[34]  Karl M Newell,et al.  Task goals and change in dynamical degrees of freedom with motor learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  K. Newell,et al.  MODELING THE CONTROL OF ISOMETRIC FORCE PRODUCTION WITH PIECE-WISE LINEAR, STOCHASTIC MAPS OF MULTIPLE TIME-SCALES , 2003 .

[36]  Gavin P. Lawrence,et al.  Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the control of movement amplitude. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[37]  K. M. Newell,et al.  Stochastic processes in postural center-of-pressure profiles , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Neil Salkind,et al.  Using SPSS for Windows and Macintosh , 2007 .