Toward an Integration of the Social and the Scientific: Observing, Modeling, and Promoting the Explanatory Coherence of Reasoning

It may seem odd for two cognitive scientists, each with little specific expertise in social psychology, to present a chapter that focuses on social cognition. Indeed, our past work may seem much more in the realm of scientific reasoning than in that of social reasoning. But one question that we have been asking, both of ourselves and of our colleagues, is, "What is the difference between 'scientific reasoning' and plain old 'reasoning'?" Generally, people hem and haw when confronted with this question, then speak of the latter as if it were social reasoning—and quite often, they mention socially based ruminations that involve suboptimal decisions, faulty heuristics, and inappropriately biased values, goals, and the like (see Gigerenzer, 1991; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974, and many others). Useful follow-up questions to such respondents include, "Well, is the difference between these two sorts of reasoning qualitative or quantitative?" Put another way (as many—including Einstein, 1950,—seem to have occasionally wondered), "Is scientific reasoning just (a) more likely to employ formal tools (like deduction or mathematics) and/or (b) more likely to involve the vigilant search for disconfirmation—something that just plain folks (Lave, 1988, p. 4) do, but less frequently? 2 Put rather bluntly, we have not been able to reject the hypothesis that the word "scientific" in "scientific reasoning" is superfluous. In an era of specialization, we realize that it is a bit out of fashion to undifferentiate reasoning (although one can argue that interdisciplinary cognitive science itself similarly bucks the trend); still, we are more struck by how much of the everyday is found in scientific reasoning (and vice versa) than by how unique scientific reasoning is. Thus, we believe that the principles of reasoning that have been seen primarily as characterizing scientific reasoning can equally well be viewed as central to social reasoning. 1 Bifurcating the set of reasoning processes into the social and the scientific is a bit like bifurcating a deity and still considering the encompassing religion to be monotheistic. In essence, adding either the modifier "social" or "scientific" seems unnecessary, unless one speaks about the domain being reasoned about (discussed later). Consider the following experience. While at a social gathering, a friend's father remarked that sugar-laden foods dramatically boost children's activity. It was obvious, he claimed, because he had watched a birthday party full of kids "go crazy" after eating the cake. Another guest (perhaps a "better reasoner" on this topic) …

[1]  J. Lave Cognition in Practice: Outdoors: a social anthropology of cognition in practice , 1988 .

[2]  S. Read,et al.  Connectionism, Parallel Constraint Satisfaction Processes, and Gestalt Principles: (Re)Introducing Cognitive Dynamics to Social Psychology , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[3]  Christopher Hoadley,et al.  "I Know One When I See One": How (Much) Do Hypotheses Differ from Evidence? , 1994 .

[4]  P. Tetlock Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. , 1985 .

[5]  P. Thagard,et al.  Probabilistic Networks and Explanatory Coherence. , 1997 .

[6]  L. J. Shrum,et al.  The recycling of solid wastes: Personal values, value orientations, and attitudes about recycling as antecedents of recycling behavior , 1994 .

[7]  Paul Thagard,et al.  Deliberative coherence , 2004, Synthese.

[8]  A. Einstein Out of My Later Years , 1950 .

[9]  G. Gigerenzer From Tools to Theories: A Heuristic of Discovery in Cognitive Psychology. , 1991 .

[10]  M. Ranney,et al.  Assessing Explanatory Coherence : A New Method for Integrating Verbal Data with Models of On-line Belief Revision , 1992 .

[11]  M. Ranney,et al.  Computational tools for modeling and aiding reasoning: assessing and applying the theory of explanatory coherence , 1995 .

[12]  Paul Thagard,et al.  Explanatory Coherence and Belief Revision in Naive Physics , 1988 .

[13]  M. Ranney,et al.  Ecological behavior, environmental attitude, and feelings of responsibility for the environment , 1999 .

[14]  P. Thagard,et al.  Explanatory coherence , 1993 .

[15]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[16]  J. Bargh,et al.  The psychology of action : linking cognition and motivation to behavior , 1999 .

[17]  G. Lakoff Women, fire, and dangerous things : what categories reveal about the mind , 1989 .

[18]  L. Wheeler,et al.  Review of personality and social psychology , 1980 .

[19]  R. Samuels Mind: Introduction to Cognitive Science, Paul Thagard, MIT Press (1996), ISBN: 0 262 20106 2 , 1997 .

[20]  E. Miller,et al.  Headache , 1980 .

[21]  T. Shultz,et al.  Cognitive dissonance reduction as constraint satisfaction. , 1996, Psychological review.

[22]  Robert Hogan,et al.  Fifty years of personality psychology , 1993 .

[23]  Carl Bereiter,et al.  Implications of Connectionism for Thinking about Rules , 1991 .

[24]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[25]  S. Read,et al.  Explanatory coherence in social explanations : a parallel distributed processing account , 1993 .

[26]  G. Fletcher The Scientific Credibility of Commonsense Psychology , 1993 .

[27]  T. Shultz,et al.  A Constraint Satisfaction Model of Cognitive Dissonance Phenomena , 2000 .

[28]  Lawrence A. Pervin,et al.  Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. , 1989 .

[29]  L. Kahle Social values and social change: Adaptation to life in America , 1983 .

[30]  P. Stern,et al.  Psychological research for the new energy problems: Strategies and opportunities. , 1992 .

[31]  N. Chater,et al.  Proceedings of the fourteenth annual conference of the cognitive science society , 1992 .

[32]  P. Thagard,et al.  FORMING IMPRESSIONS FROM STEREOTYPES, TRAITS, AND BEHAVIORS : A PARALLEL-CONSTRAINT-SATISFACTION THEORY , 1996 .

[33]  L. Ross,et al.  Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. , 1981 .

[34]  Patricia Schank,et al.  The Psychological Fidelity of ECHO : Modeling an Experimental Study of Explanatory Coherence , 2003 .

[35]  Allen Newell,et al.  Human Problem Solving. , 1973 .

[36]  N. Pennington,et al.  Explanation-based decision making: effects of memory structure on judgment , 1988 .

[37]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind , 1988 .

[38]  Christopher M. Hoadley,et al.  Wander ECHO: A Connectionist Simulation of Limited Coherence , 2019, Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[39]  Fern K. Willits,et al.  Environmental Attitudes and Behavior , 1994 .

[40]  Robert Dale,et al.  Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1991 .

[41]  H. Simon,et al.  A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice , 1955 .

[42]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Things That Make Us Smart: Defending Human Attributes In The Age Of The Machine , 1993 .

[43]  David N. Perkins,et al.  Outsmarting IQ: The Emerging Science of Learnable Intelligence , 1995 .

[44]  S. Read,et al.  Rapist or "Regular Guy": Explanatory Coherence in the Construction of Mental Models of Others , 1993 .

[45]  Keith J. Holyoak,et al.  A Coherence Model of Cognitive Consistency: Dynamics of Attitude Change During the Persian Gulf War , 1993 .

[46]  Christine Diehl,et al.  Multiple Representations for Improving Scientific Thinking , 1995 .

[47]  P. Tetlock The Impact of Accountability on Judgment and Choice: Toward A Social Contingency Model , 1992 .