How best to improve upon return-to-player information in gambling? A comparison of two approaches in an Australian sample

Abstract “Return-to-player” information is used in several jurisdictions to display the long-run cost of gambling, but previous evidence suggests that these messages are frequently misunderstood by gamblers. Two ways of improving the communication of return-to-player information have been suggested: switching to an equivalent “house-edge” format, or via the use of a “volatility warning,” clarifying that the information applies only in the statistical long run. In this study, Australian participants (N = 603) were presented with either a standard return-to-player message, the same message supplemented with a volatility warning, or a house-edge message. The return-to-player plus volatility warning message was understood correctly more frequently than the return-to-player message, but the house-edge message was understood best of all. Participants perceived the lowest chance of winning in the return-to-player plus volatility warning condition. These findings contribute data on the relative merits of two proposed approaches in the design of improved gambling information.

[1]  Elliot A. Ludvig,et al.  Impact of the "when the fun stops, stop" gambling message on online gambling behaviour: a randomised, online experimental study. , 2022, The Lancet. Public health.

[2]  P. Newall,et al.  House-edge information and a volatility warning lead to reduced gambling expenditure: Potential improvements to return-to-player percentages. , 2022, Addictive behaviors.

[3]  Elliot A. Ludvig,et al.  Risk communication improvements for gambling: House-edge information and volatility statements. , 2020, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[4]  Elliot A. Ludvig,et al.  Gambling warning labels: Differing effects of message length and message format , 2020 .

[5]  Elliot A. Ludvig,et al.  Equivalent gambling warning labels are perceived differently , 2020, Addiction.

[6]  Ben J. Smith,et al.  Identifying effective policy interventions to prevent gambling-related harm. , 2019 .

[7]  A. C. Walker,et al.  Graphical Depiction of Statistical Information Improves Gambling-Related Judgments , 2019, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[8]  A. Blaszczynski,et al.  Return-to-Player Percentage in Gaming Machines: Impact of Informative Materials on Player Understanding , 2019, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[9]  D. Hodgins,et al.  Obtaining quality data using behavioral measures of impulsivity in gambling research with Amazon’s Mechanical Turk , 2018, Journal of behavioral addictions.

[10]  M. Browne,et al.  Exploring the Effectiveness of an Intelligent Messages Framework for Developing Warning Messages to Reduce Gambling Intensity , 2018, Journal of Gambling Issues.

[11]  J. Whelan,et al.  Warning messages for electronic gambling machines: evidence for regulatory policies , 2017 .

[12]  A. Blaszczynski,et al.  Key issues in product-based harmm minimisation: examining theory, evidence and policy issues relevant inGreat Britain , 2017 .

[13]  M. Abbott,et al.  The Effects of Pop-up Harm Minimisation Messages on Electronic Gaming Machine Gambling Behaviour in New Zealand , 2016, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[14]  Richard Woolley,et al.  House edge: hold percentage and the cost of EGM gambling , 2013 .

[15]  M. Rockloff Validation of the Consumption Screen for Problem Gambling (CSPG) , 2012, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[16]  N. Turner Volatility, House Edge and Prize Structure of Gambling Games , 2011, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[17]  A. Blaszczynski,et al.  Do Warning Signs on Electronic Gaming Machines Influence Irrational Cognitions? , 2009, Psychological reports.

[18]  M. Dixon,et al.  PAR Sheets, probabilities, and slot machine play: Implications for problem and non-problem gambling , 2009 .

[19]  Serge Sévigny,et al.  Responsible Gambling Tools: Pop-Up Messages and Pauses on Video Lottery Terminals , 2006, The Journal of psychology.