Accuracy of Intercepting Moving Tactile Targets

When intercepting a moving target, we typically rely on vision to determine where the target is and where it will soon be. The accuracy of visually guided interception can be represented by a model that combines the perceived position and velocity of the target to estimate when and where to hit it and guides the finger accordingly with a short delay. We might expect the accuracy of interception to similarly depend on haptic judgments of position and velocity. To test this, we conducted separate experiments to measure the precision and any biases in tactile perception of position and velocity and used our findings to predict the precision and biases that would be present in an interception task if it were performed according to the principle described earlier. We then performed a tactile interception task to test our predictions. We found that interception of tactile targets is guided by similar principles as interception of visual targets.

[1]  Claudio de’Sperati,et al.  Speed Biases With Real-Life Video Clips , 2018, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[2]  E. Kalko,et al.  Echolocating Bats Cry Out Loud to Detect Their Prey , 2008, PloS one.

[3]  Alessandro Moscatelli,et al.  Implied Dynamics Biases the Visual Perception of Velocity , 2014, PloS one.

[4]  Luigi Tamè,et al.  More Than Skin-Deep: Integration of Skin-Based and Musculoskeletal Reference Frames in Localization of Touch , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Gerald Westheimer,et al.  Discrimination of short time intervals by the human observer , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Peter U. Tse Transformational apparent motion in the volume domain , 1999 .

[7]  K. Drewing The Extent of Skin Bending Rather Than Action Possibilities Explains Why Holes Feel Larger With the Tongue Than With the Finger , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Joan López-Moliner,et al.  Hitting moving targets with a continuously changing temporal window , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Prediction of a moving target's position in fast goal-directed action , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[10]  Joan López-Moliner,et al.  Dealing with delays does not transfer across sensorimotor tasks. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[11]  Monica Gori,et al.  Spatial Representation of the Workspace in Blind, Low Vision, and Sighted Human Participants , 2018, i-Perception.

[12]  Ts Troscianko,et al.  Perceived speed of urban traffic , 1999 .

[13]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Potential Systematic Interception Errors are Avoided When Tracking the Target with One’s Eyes , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[14]  Timothy C. Tricas Bioelectric-Mediated Predation by Swell Sharks, Cephaloscyllium ventriosum , 1982 .

[15]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Behavioral dynamics of intercepting a moving target , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Specificity of Reflex Adaptation for Task-Relevant Variability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Ellen Poliakoff,et al.  Tactile spatial acuity varies with site and axis in the human upper limb , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  E. Poulton Models for biases in judging sensory magnitude. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  B. Green The perception of distance and location for dual tactile pressures , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  E. Brenner,et al.  Hitting moving targets Continuous control of the acceleration of the hand on the basis of the target’s velocity , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  J. Randall Flanagan,et al.  A Rapid Tactile-Motor Reflex Automatically Guides Reaching toward Handheld Objects , 2016, Current Biology.

[22]  T. Heed,et al.  Reach Trajectories Characterize Tactile Localization for Sensorimotor Decision Making , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast Responses of the Human Hand to Changes in Target Position. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast and fine-tuned corrections when the target of a hand movement is displaced , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Tatjana Seizova-Cajic,et al.  Biases in tactile localization by pointing: compression for weak stimuli and centering for distributions of stimuli. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Walter M. X. Zimmer,et al.  Beaked whales echolocate on prey , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Multisensory integration: psychophysics, neurophysiology, and computation , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[28]  Igor Dolgov,et al.  Blind(fold)ed by science: A constant target-heading angle is used in visual and nonvisual pursuit , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[29]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control , 2022 .

[30]  Eli Brenner,et al.  How Can People Be so Good at Intercepting Accelerating Objects if They Are so Poor at Visually Judging Acceleration? , 2016, i-Perception.

[31]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  When Up Is Down in 0g: How Gravity Sensing Affects the Timing of Interceptive Actions , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Claire F. Michaels,et al.  Lateral ball interception: hand movements during linear ball trajectories , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  M. A. MacIver,et al.  Prey capture in the weakly electric fish Apteronotus albifrons: sensory acquisition strategies and electrosensory consequences. , 1999, The Journal of experimental biology.

[34]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Errors in interception can be predicted from errors in perception , 2018, Cortex.

[35]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Robust movement segmentation by combining multiple sources of information , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[36]  Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al.  Errors in visuo-haptic and haptic-haptic location matching are stable over long periods of time. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[37]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Sensory integration does not lead to sensory calibration , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Heiko Wagner,et al.  Analyzing the kinematics of hand movements in catching tasks—An online correction analysis of movement toward the target’s trajectory , 2017, Behavior Research Methods.

[39]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Continuously updating one's predictions underlies successful interception. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Eli Brenner,et al.  How people achieve their amazing temporal precision in interception. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[41]  Leonie Oostwoud Wijdenes,et al.  Movement Adjustments Have Short Latencies Because There is No Need to Detect Anything. , 2016, Motor control.