Articulation Effects in Lightness: Historical Background and Theoretical Implications

The concept of articulation was first introduced by Katz [1935 The World of Colour (London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co)] to refer to the degree of complexity within a field. Katz, who created the basic research methods for studying lightness constancy, found that the greater the degree of articulation within a field of illumination, the greater the degree of constancy. Even though this concept has been largely forgotten, there is much empirical evidence for Katz's principle, and the effects on lightness are very strong. However, when articulation is increased within a framework that does not coincide with a region of illumination, constancy is weakened. Kardos (1934 Zeitschrift für Psychologie Ergänzungband 23) advanced the concept of co-determination, according to which the lightness of a surface is determined relative to more than one field of illumination. Gilchrist et al (1999 Psychological Review 106 795–834) argue that the fields concept should be replaced by the more operational frameworks concept and that a wide variety of lightness errors can be explained by a modification of the Katz principle: the greater the articulation within a perceptual framework, the stronger the anchoring of lightness values within that framework.

[1]  W. Benary,et al.  Beobachtungen zu einem Experiment über Helligkeitskontrast , 1924 .

[2]  G. Katona Zur Analyse der Helligkeitskonstanz , 1929 .

[3]  A. Gelb Die „Farbenkonstanz“ der Sehdinge , 1929 .

[4]  Robert H. Thouless,et al.  Phenomenal Regression to the Real Object , 1931, Nature.

[5]  W. Peddie Phenomenal Regression to the Real Object , 1933, Nature.

[6]  K. Koffka Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .

[7]  D. Katz The world of colour , 1935 .

[8]  R. Henneman,et al.  A photometric study of the perception of object color , 1935 .

[9]  H. Helson,et al.  Adaptation-level theory , 1964 .

[10]  D. Jameson,et al.  Theory of brightness and color contrast in human vision. , 1964, Vision research.

[11]  Eric G. Heinemann,et al.  Simultaneous Brightness Induction , 1972 .

[12]  A. Gilchrist Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement. , 1977, Science.

[13]  A. Gilchrist The perception of surface blacks and whites. , 1979, Scientific American.

[14]  M White,et al.  The Effect of the Nature of the Surround on the Perceived Lightness of Grey Bars within Square-Wave Test Gratings , 1981, Perception.

[15]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  The classification and integration of edges as critical to the perception of reflectance and illumination , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  L. Arend,et al.  Simultaneous constancy, lightness, and brightness. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[17]  L. Arend,et al.  Perceived lightness, but not brightness, of achromatic surfaces depends on perceived depth information , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  L. Arend,et al.  Illumination change at a depth edge can reduce lightness constancy , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  J. Frisby,et al.  The Role of 3-D Surface Slope in a Lightness/Brightness Effect , 1997, Vision Research.

[20]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999, Psychological review.

[21]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  Relative area and relative luminance combine to anchor surface lightness values , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  E. Adelson Lightness Perception and Lightness Illusions , 1999 .

[23]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999 .