Evaluating the feasibility of the steady-state visual evoked potential (SSVEP) to study temporal attention.

Improvements in perceptual performance can be obtained when events in the environment are temporally predictable-and temporal predictability improves attention and sensory processing. The amplitude of the steady-state visual evoked potential (SSVEP) has been shown to correlate with attention paid to a flickering stimulus even if the flickering stimulus is irrelevant for the task. However, to our knowledge, the validity of the SSVEP to study temporal attention has not been established. Therefore, we designed an SSVEP temporal attention task to evaluate whether the SSVEP and its temporal dynamics can be used to study temporal attention. We used a forced-choice perceptual detection task while presenting task-irrelevant visual flicker at alpha (10 Hz) and two surrounding frequencies (6 or 15 Hz). Temporal predictability was manipulated by having the interstimulus intervals (ISI) be constant or variable. Behavioral results replicated previous studies confirming the benefits of temporal expectations on performance for trials with constant ISI. EEG analyses revealed robust SSVEP amplitudes for all flicker frequencies, although a main effect of temporal expectations on SSVEP amplitude was not significant. Additional analyses revealed temporal predictability-related modulations of SSVEP amplitude at 10 Hz and its second harmonic (20 Hz). The effect of temporal predictability was also observed for the 6 Hz flicker, but not for 15 Hz for any ISI condition. These results provide some evidence for the feasibility of the SSVEP technique to study temporal attention for stimuli with flicker frequencies around the alpha band.

[1]  Daniel J. Acheson,et al.  Increased Alpha-Band Power during the Retention of Shapes and Shape-Location Associations in Visual Short-Term Memory , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[2]  A. Sack,et al.  Alpha-Band Rhythms in Visual Task Performance: Phase-Locking by Rhythmic Sensory Stimulation , 2013, PloS one.

[3]  A. Nobre,et al.  Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[5]  A. Nobre,et al.  Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  Annabelle Blangero,et al.  Exploiting individual primary visual cortex geometry to boost steady state visual evoked potentials , 2013, Journal of neural engineering.

[7]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials , 2006, Brain Research.

[8]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  G. Sperling,et al.  Attentional modulation of SSVEP power depends on the network tagged by the flicker frequency. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  A. Nobre,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Related to Anticipatory Attention Follow Temporal Expectations , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Concurrent recording of steady-state and transient event-related potentials as indices of visual-spatial selective attention , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[12]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Attention induces synchronization-based response gain in steady-state visual evoked potentials , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Combining spatial and temporal expectations to improve visual perception. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[14]  Ramesh Srinivasan,et al.  Steady-State Visual Evoked Potentials: Distributed Local Sources and Wave-Like Dynamics Are Sensitive to Flicker Frequency , 2006, Brain Topography.

[15]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  M. Belmonte,et al.  Shifts of visual spatial attention modulate a steady-state visual evoked potential. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  Joseph Ciorciari,et al.  Steady-State Visually Evoked Potential topography associated with a visual vigilance task , 2005, Brain Topography.

[18]  K. Mathewson,et al.  To See or Not to See: Prestimulus α Phase Predicts Visual Awareness , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  M Tagliati,et al.  The pattern electroretinogram in Parkinson's disease reveals lack of retinal spatial tuning. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[21]  A. Nobre,et al.  Multiple mechanisms of selective attention: differential modulation of stimulus processing by attention to space or time , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  C. Bundesen,et al.  Temporal expectancy in the context of a theory of visual attention , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Bin He,et al.  SSVEP signatures of binocular rivalry during simultaneous EEG and fMRI , 2015, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[24]  A. Cichocki,et al.  Steady-state visually evoked potentials: Focus on essential paradigms and future perspectives , 2010, Progress in Neurobiology.

[25]  R. VanRullen,et al.  Spontaneous EEG oscillations reveal periodic sampling of visual attention , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20-28 Hz range. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[27]  K. Mathewson,et al.  Rescuing stimuli from invisibility: Inducing a momentary release from visual masking with pre-target entrainment , 2010, Cognition.

[28]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Look who's talking: The deployment of visuo-spatial attention during multisensory speech processing under noisy environmental conditions , 2008, NeuroImage.

[29]  R. McCarley,et al.  Attentional cues in chronic schizophrenia: abnormal disengagement of attention. , 1992, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[30]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Rhythmic entrainment source separation: Optimizing analyses of neural responses to rhythmic sensory stimulation , 2016, NeuroImage.

[31]  Verena C. Seibold,et al.  Toward the influence of temporal attention on the selection of targets in a visual search task: An ERP study. , 2016, Psychophysiology.

[32]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Selective attention to stimulus location modulates the steady-state visual evoked potential. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  Lynn Rochester,et al.  The effect of external rhythmic cues (auditory and visual) on walking during a functional task in homes of people with Parkinson's disease. , 2005, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[34]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[35]  Jennifer T. Coull,et al.  Behavioural Dissociation between Exogenous and Endogenous Temporal Orienting of Attention , 2011, PloS one.

[36]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Combining steady‐state visual evoked potentials and f MRI to localize brain activity during selective attention , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[37]  Febo Cincotti,et al.  Sub-second "temporal attention" modulates alpha rhythms. A high-resolution EEG study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[38]  A. Keil,et al.  Temporal Trade-Off Effects in Sustained Attention: Dynamics in Visual Cortex Predict the Target Detection Performance during Distraction , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Ramesh Srinivasan,et al.  Distinct Attention Networks for Feature Enhancement and Suppression in Vision , 2012, Psychological science.

[40]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Age-Related Changes in Orienting Attention in Time , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  M. Woldorff,et al.  Attentional capacity for processing concurrent stimuli is larger across sensory modalities than within a modality. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[42]  S. Andersen,et al.  Behavioral performance follows the time course of neural facilitation and suppression during cued shifts of feature-selective attention , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  Justin M. Ales,et al.  The steady-state visual evoked potential in vision research: A review. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[44]  C. Umilta,et al.  Deficits of the automatic orienting of attention in schizophrenic patients. , 1993, Journal of psychiatric research.

[45]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[46]  S. Andersen,et al.  Effects of overt and covert attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[47]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Attentional preparation based on temporal expectancy modulates processing at the perceptual level , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Sarah M Cormiea,et al.  Winter is coming: How humans forage in a temporally structured environment. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[49]  C. Herrmann Human EEG responses to 1–100 Hz flicker: resonance phenomena in visual cortex and their potential correlation to cognitive phenomena , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Human Neuroscience , 2022 .

[51]  L. Craighero,et al.  Electromyographic Activity of Hand Muscles in a Motor Coordination Game: Effect of Incentive Scheme and Its Relation with Social Capital , 2011, PloS one.

[52]  Anna C. Nobre,et al.  Synergistic Effect of Combined Temporal and Spatial Expectations on Visual Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  S A Hillyard,et al.  Feature-selective attention enhances color signals in early visual areas of the human brain , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Francesca Pei,et al.  Neural correlates of object-based attention. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[55]  Hedderik van Rijn,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms underlying individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2014, NeuroImage.

[56]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Prolonged reduction of electrocortical activity predicts correct performance during rapid serial visual processing. , 2009, Psychophysiology.

[57]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Predictive knowledge of stimulus relevance does not influence top-down suppression of irrelevant information in older adults , 2010, Cortex.

[58]  A. Nobre,et al.  Endogenous modulation of low frequency oscillations by temporal expectations , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Orienting Attention in Time Activates Left Intraparietal Sulcus for Both Perceptual and Motor Task Goals , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  Min-Chul Park,et al.  SSVEP and ERP measurement of cognitive fatigue caused by stereoscopic 3D , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[62]  Floris P. de Lange,et al.  Local Entrainment of Alpha Oscillations by Visual Stimuli Causes Cyclic Modulation of Perception , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  R. Silberstein,et al.  Steady-state visually evoked potential topography during the continuous performance task in normal controls and schizophrenia , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[64]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Temporal expectation and information processing: A model-based analysis , 2012, Cognition.

[66]  Alejandro Lleras,et al.  Making Waves in the Stream of Consciousness: Entraining Oscillations in EEG Alpha and Fluctuations in Visual Awareness with Rhythmic Visual Stimulation , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[67]  F. Freeman Experimental analysis of the writing movement. , 1914 .

[68]  J. Pernier,et al.  Stimulus Specificity of Phase-Locked and Non-Phase-Locked 40 Hz Visual Responses in Human , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  Donald J Bolger,et al.  The neurophysiological bases of EEG and EEG measurement: a review for the rest of us. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[70]  Verena C. Seibold,et al.  Does temporal preparation speed up visual processing? Evidence from the N2pc. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[71]  A. Stockman,et al.  Color and brightness encoded in a common L- and M-cone pathway with expansive and compressive nonlinearities. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[72]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[73]  Marc M. Van Hulle,et al.  Language Model Applications to Spelling with Brain-Computer Interfaces , 2014, Sensors.

[74]  Hongjing Lu,et al.  Two forms of aftereffects induced by transparent motion reveal multilevel adaptation. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[75]  Dewen Hu,et al.  Retinotopic and topographic analyses with gaze restriction for steady-state visual evoked potentials , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[76]  C Miniussi,et al.  Orienting attention in time. , 2001, Frontiers in bioscience : a journal and virtual library.

[77]  Verena C. Seibold,et al.  Temporal attention shortens perceptual latency: a temporal prior entry effect. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[78]  Kazumichi Matsumiya,et al.  Time Courses of Attentional Modulation in Neural Amplification and Synchronization Measured with Steady-state Visual-evoked Potentials , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.