How Does Experience Modulate Auditory Spatial Processing in Individuals with Blindness?

Abstract Comparing early- and late-onset blindness in individuals offers a unique model for studying the influence of visual experience on neural processing. This study investigated how prior visual experience would modulate auditory spatial processing among blind individuals. BOLD responses of early- and late-onset blind participants were captured while performing a sound localization task. The task required participants to listen to novel “Bat-ears” sounds, analyze the spatial information embedded in the sounds, and specify out of 15 locations where the sound would have been emitted. In addition to sound localization, participants were assessed on visuospatial working memory and general intellectual abilities. The results revealed common increases in BOLD responses in the middle occipital gyrus, superior frontal gyrus, precuneus, and precentral gyrus during sound localization for both groups. Between-group dissociations, however, were found in the right middle occipital gyrus and left superior frontal gyrus. The BOLD responses in the left superior frontal gyrus were significantly correlated with accuracy on sound localization and visuospatial working memory abilities among the late-onset blind participants. In contrast, the accuracy on sound localization only correlated with BOLD responses in the right middle occipital gyrus among the early-onset counterpart. The findings support the notion that early-onset blind individuals rely more on the occipital areas as a result of cross-modal plasticity for auditory spatial processing, while late-onset blind individuals rely more on the prefrontal areas which subserve visuospatial working memory.

[1]  R. Zatorre,et al.  A Functional Neuroimaging Study of Sound Localization: Visual Cortex Activity Predicts Performance in Early-Blind Individuals , 2005, PLoS biology.

[2]  G. Vandewalle,et al.  Functional specialization for auditory–spatial processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind humans , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Cesare Cornoldi,et al.  Visuo-spatial working memory: Structures and variables affecting a capacity measure , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[6]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Differential occipital responses in early- and late-blind individuals during a sound-source discrimination task , 2008, NeuroImage.

[7]  Deanne M. Doorlag,et al.  Cassette Braille: A New Communication Tool for Blind People , 1983 .

[8]  O. Collignon,et al.  Functional selectivity in sensory-deprived cortices. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Generalisability, Random Effects & Population Inference , 1998, NeuroImage.

[10]  R. M. Sachs,et al.  Anthropometric manikin for acoustic research. , 1975, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  Margaret Ross,et al.  Self-Reported Spatial Hearing and Blindness , 2007 .

[12]  M. Davare,et al.  Reorganisation of the Right Occipito-Parietal Stream for Auditory Spatial Processing in Early Blind Humans. A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2009, Brain Topography.

[13]  D. Wechsler Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. , 1955 .

[14]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[15]  Hubert D. Zimmer,et al.  Common coding of auditory and visual spatial information in working memory , 2008, Brain Research.

[16]  David Medyckyj-Scott,et al.  Human Factors in Geographical Information Systems , 1994 .

[17]  R J Ilmoniemi,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for cross-modal plasticity in humans with early- and late-onset blindness. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[18]  M. Eimer Multisensory Integration: How Visual Experience Shapes Spatial Perception , 2004, Current Biology.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Different activation patterns in the visual cortex of late and congenitally blind subjects. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  M. Raichle,et al.  Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a FMRI study of verb generation to heard nouns. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Detecting Activations in PET and fMRI: Levels of Inference and Power , 1996, NeuroImage.

[22]  Factor analysis of the mainland Chinese version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-RC) in a brain-damaged sample. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[23]  R. Saxe,et al.  A sensitive period for language in the visual cortex: Distinct patterns of plasticity in congenitally versus late blind adults , 2011, Brain and Language.

[24]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. , 1998, Science.

[25]  Sandra M. Sanabria-Bohórquez,et al.  Occipital Activation by Pattern Recognition in the Early Blind Using Auditory Substitution for Vision , 2001, NeuroImage.

[26]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[27]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Relevance of Spectral Cues for Auditory Spatial Processing in the Occipital Cortex of the Blind , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[28]  Hubert D Zimmer,et al.  Auditory and visual spatial working memory , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[29]  S Martinkauppi,et al.  Working memory of auditory localization. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  Christopher S. Monk,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of spatial working memory in children. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[31]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Sensitive Period for a Multimodal Response in Human Visual Motion Area MT/MST , 2010, Current Biology.

[32]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense? , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  C. Cornoldi,et al.  Individual differences in the capacity limitations of visuospatial short-term memory: Research on sighted and totally congenitally blind people , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[34]  C. J. Kolk Intelligence Testing for Visually Impaired Persons , 1977 .

[35]  A. Thierry,et al.  Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex , 1994, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.

[36]  C. Veraart,et al.  Functional Cerebral Reorganization for Auditory Spatial Processing and Auditory Substitution of Vision in Early Blind Subjects , 2006 .

[37]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  Multisensory perceptual learning reshapes both fast and slow mechanisms of crossmodal processing , 2011, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[38]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  The Issue of Memory in the Study of Prefrontal Function , 1994 .

[39]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[40]  Manabu Honda,et al.  Critical Period for Cross-Modal Plasticity in Blind Humans: A Functional MRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[41]  Morton A. Heller,et al.  Touch, representation, and blindness , 2000 .

[42]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  A Positron Emission Tomographic Study of Auditory Localization in the Congenitally Blind , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  M. Raichle,et al.  Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a fMRI study of Braille reading. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual experience, visual field size, and the development of nonvisual sensitivity to the spatial structure of outdoor neighborhoods explored by walking. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[45]  G. Vandewalle,et al.  Impact of blindness onset on the functional organization and the connectivity of the occipital cortex. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Multisensory spatial interactions: a window onto functional integration in the human brain , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[47]  Xiwen Sun,et al.  Age-dependent brain activation during forward and backward digit recall revealed by fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[48]  C. Nelson,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of spatial working memory in children. , 2000 .

[49]  Richard A. Salome,et al.  The Visual Experience , 1990 .

[50]  A. Anastasi Individual differences. , 2020, Annual review of psychology.

[51]  P. Pietrini,et al.  Neural correlates of spatial working memory in humans: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study comparing visual and tactile processes , 2006, Neuroscience.

[52]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[53]  M. Hallett,et al.  Period of susceptibility for cross‐modal plasticity in the blind , 1999, Annals of neurology.

[54]  Cesare Cornoldi,et al.  Mental imagery in blind people: the role of passive and active visuospatial processes , 2000 .

[55]  G. Wittenberg,et al.  Functional connectivity between somatosensory and visual cortex in early blind humans , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[56]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[58]  P. Goldman-Rakic Architecture of the Prefrontal Cortex and the Central Executive , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[59]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Preserved Functional Specialization for Spatial Processing in the Middle Occipital Gyrus of the Early Blind , 2010, Neuron.

[60]  Alex W. K. Wong,et al.  Cross auditory‐spatial learning in early‐blind individuals , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[61]  Stephen R. Arnott,et al.  The auditory dorsal pathway: Orienting vision , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.