Binocular brightness combinations: Additive and nonadditive aspects

A conjoint measurement procedure is used for the measurement of binocular brightness as a function of left and right luminance inputs. For nonzero stimulation, the data confirm earlier findings: the system can be described as additive with a scale exponent of 1. If zero stimulation is included, however, no additive solution can be found (due to Fechner’s paradox). This fact, combined with various critical remarks in the literature with respect to the existence of a real luminance-averaging system, has led us to propose a model which takes account of Fechner’s paradox, and incorporates “realistic” exponents without requiring a multistage processing mechanism where different levels are characterized by different sensory scales. The proposed model makes the weighting coefficients for the two eyes dependent in a continuous way on the strength of stimulation in the two eyes, especially on the amount of contrast of the monocular stimuli. For zero background stimulation, contrast can be expressed in terms of luminance of the stimulus. In this way, the model is reduced to a simple testable form. While it much simpler than Engel’s (1969) model, the experimental results indicate that it might also work for the more general case.

[1]  G. Fechner Über einige Verhältnisse des binocularen Sehens , 2022 .

[2]  W. Smith The Integrative Action of the Nervous System , 1907, Nature.

[3]  Lehrbuch der Physik , 1913 .

[4]  F. Stratton Müller-Pouillets Lehrbuch der Physik , 1928, Nature.

[5]  S. H. Bartley,et al.  SUMMATION AND SUBTRACTION OF BRIGHTNESS IN BINOCULAR PERCEPTION , 1930 .

[6]  S. H. Bartley,et al.  The Brilliance of an Object Seen Binocularly , 1933 .

[7]  Muller-Pouillets Lehrbuch der Physik , 1934, Nature.

[8]  Binocular brightness summation. , 1950, Optical journal and review of optometry.

[9]  Bartley Sh,et al.  Binocular brightness summation. , 1950 .

[10]  Ewald Hering Outlines of a theory of the light sense , 1964 .

[11]  Herbert F. Crovitz Retinal Locus in Tachistoscopic Binocular Color Rivalry , 1964, Perceptual and motor skills.

[12]  Micheal Treisman,et al.  A statistical decision model for sensory discrimination which predicts Weber’s law and other sensory laws: Some results of a computer simulation , 1966 .

[13]  Micheal Treisman,et al.  A statistical decision model for sensory discrimination which predicts Weber’s law and other sensory laws: Some results of a computer simulation , 1966 .

[14]  E. Galanter,et al.  Brightnesses, luminances, and Fechner’s paradox , 1967 .

[15]  G. R. Engel The visual processes underlying binocular brightness summation. , 1967, Vision research.

[16]  A. Vendrik,et al.  Differential luminance sensitivity of the human visual system , 1969 .

[17]  G. R. Engel,et al.  The autocorrelation function and binocular brightness mixing. , 1969, Vision research.

[18]  M. Treisman Brightness contrast and the perceptual scale. , 1970, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[19]  D. Jameson,et al.  The perception of brightness and darkness , 1970 .

[20]  D M Green,et al.  A neural timing theory for response times and the psychophysics of intensity. , 1972, Psychological review.

[21]  D. Macleod,et al.  The Schrödinger Equation in Binocular Brightness Combination , 1972, Perception.

[22]  L. Marks Brightness and equivalent intensity of intrinsic light. , 1973, Vision research.