ARTICULATORY PROSODIES IN GERMAN REDUCED SPEECH
暂无分享,去创建一个
Starting from spontaneous speech data of the Kiel Corpus, reduct-ion patterns of function words are described, which also incorporate more global articulatory prosodies, such as nasality, labiality and glottalization. The results of 4 perceptual experiments support the hypothesis that these long components of speech production are mapped onto perception. The discussion is also a contribution to a new paradigm for the analysis of non-lab and non-scripted speech. 1. PRODUCTION PATTERNS OF FUNCTION WORDS 1.1. Articulatory Fusion and Lexicalization The phonetic form of function words in German shows great variability along an articulatory scale from strong elaboration to a high degree of reduction [7, 8], depending, among other factors, on prosody, especially sentence accent as well as prosodic grouping, and context of situation. In sequences of unstressed function words, reduction may result in their articulatory fusion, e.g., of prepositions with articles (a) or of auxiliary verbs with enclitic pronouns (b). This may lead to the emergence of new lexical items, for example, in category (a), to monosyllabic “zum” [tsUm] (“to the”) by the side of bisyllabic “zu dem”, which varies along the reduction scale from [tsu_ de_m] to [tsU bm]. Although related historically and with regard to articulatory reduction, the monoand bisyllabic phonetic forms today pattern differently in phrasal collocation due to the lexicalization of “zum”, as in “er kam zum Schluß” (“he came at/to the end”) vs. “er kam zu dem Schluß, daß...” (“he reached the conclusion that...”). In the parallel case “mit dem” (“with the”), on the other hand, articulatory fusion to monosyllabic [mIm], beyond the bisyllabic reduction scale from [mIt de_m] to [mI bm], does not result in a new lexicalization. Similarly, “haben/können/sind/sollen/wollen wir” (“have/can/ are/shall/will we”), in category (b), vary along the scales from [ha_b_n vi__]/[k_n_n vi__]/[zInt vi__]/[z_l_n vi__]/[v_l_n vi__] to [ham_]/[k_m_]/[zIm_]/[z_m_]/[v_m_], resulting in a new inflect-ional paradigm of fused lexical items that are all bisyllabic. This reduction tendency is particularly strong in spontaneous speech. 1.2. Articulatory Prosodies of Nasality and Labiality The Kiel Corpus [2] contains the example “nun wollen wir mal kucken” (“now let’s see”) in the phonetic form [nu) _ _ ) n_ V_) Ma _kHUkN] for unreduced [nu_n v_l_n vi__ ma_l _kHUkN] [7, 9, 10]. It has strong nasalization across its first three syllables relating to syllable-final nasal consonants, which are reduced (deleted or shortened) in this hypo as against the hyper pronunciation. There is additional labiodentalization around the third syllable representing canonical [v] of wir. Other possible realizations are [nu) _ _ ) (M/m)_ ) ma _ kHUkN] [10], where the apical gesture of the medial nasal is also eliminated or the consonant deleted altogether. So in these fusions of function words articulatory residues may persist as non-linear, suprasegmental features of syllables, reflecting, e.g., nasality or labiality that is no longer tied to specific segmental units. In those cases where the vowel in the first syllable bears a close acoustic relationship to the vowel in the second syllable, i.e. [_ _] [_ _] in [k_m_]/[z_m_]/[v_m_], the reduction can go further to a nasalised monosyllabic realization [k_) (_)]/[z_ ) (_ )]/[v_ ) (_)], in, e.g., “k_nnen/sollen/wollen wir das machen” (“can/shall/will we do this”). Thus “nun wollen wir mal kucken” may also be expected to be realised as [nu) _ _ ) (_) ma _kHUkN]. So nasalization may be the only articulatory parameter left to differentiate the production of “sollen wir das machen” [z_ ) _ ) das max_ n] from “soll er das machen” [z_ _ das max_ n] (“is he to do it”), which lacks it. The same nasal/oral dichotomy may apply to “sollen sie” [z_)zi] (“are they to”) vs. [z_zi] (“is she to”). 1.3. Phonatory Prosody of Glottalization Besides the articulatory prosodies of nasality and labiality, the verb-al paradigm also makes use of glottalization to differentiate “könnten/sollten/wollten wir/sie” (“could/should/would we/they”) vs. “können/sollen/wollen wir/sie”. Instead of stopping the air stream for [t] by velic action in a nasal context, the velum may be lowered throughout, and the signalling of a break, as for a plosive, may then be achieved by glottal activity, in the extreme case by a glottal stop, but irregular glottal vibration is equally possible at any point during the nasal segment [5]. Thus [k _m0_]/[z_m0_]/[v_m0_], [k _n0 zi]/[z_n0 zi]/[v_n0 zi] (alternatively with an additional syllabic modal-voice nasal after the glottalized part) are possible realizations. Furthermore, the nasal stop articulation may again be replaced by syllable nasality overlaying at least the first vowel and the glottalization then associated with its final section, as in [k_)0 zi]/[z_0) zi]/[v_)0 zi]. That is less likely to occur in the context of “wir” because in more hyper production this type of glottalization is con-nected with an oral occlusion, and therefore presupposes a conson-antal gesture, as in the position before [zi]. But in [m0_], after a vowel, the elimination of a labial closing movement leads to vowel-internal glottalization. However, the consonantal link remains and the probability of occurrence increases if, within a frame of global syllable nasality, glottalization is coupled with a lip gesture into and out of an approximant stricture, e.g. in [z_ ) V) 0 _ ) ] or [z_ ) B0 )_ )]. page 89 ICPhS99 San Francisco The distinction between a consonantal and a vocalic base of glottalization plays an important role in German phonology. An example of the former is the replacement of plosives in a sonorant, especially nasal environment, as outlined above; the latter functions as a word-initial boundary marker. The different vocal tract reson-ances for the irregular glottal pulses in the two cases are illustrated in the spectrographic analysis of “wir könnten ihn fragen” (“we could ask him”) [vi_ k_ nn0 n i0 _ n _ fra_ gN] in Figure 1. Glottalization to mark vowel onset may start in a preceding sonorant configur-ation, but this “overspill” is much shorter than the actual vocalic-base glottalization, e.g. in “wir können ihn fragen” (“we can ask him”) [vi_ k_ nn i0 _ n _ fra_ gN] of Figure 2. 1.4. Reduction Rules Reduction of function words in German exhibits patterns which can be formulated in the following rules by reference to accented strong citation form pronunciations: (1) The degree of reduction depends on word class, morphol-ogical, syntactic and prosodic structures as well as speaking style. It is particularly high for articles and their combinations with prepositions as well as for enclitic sequences of auxiliary verbs and pronouns, in certain cases resulting in new lexicalizations. (2) Diphthongs tend towards monophthongization, long vowels towards shortening and all vowels towards more central and mid positions. In extreme cases the result is [_], or [_] when phonological /r/ is involved: “ein” [_n], “der” [d_], “wir” [v_], “mit dem” [mId/t_m], “zum” [ts_m], “zur” [ts_]. (3) The glottal word boundary marker of an initial vowel may be eliminated inside unaccented article + preposition and auxiliary + enclitic pronoun constructions: “auf einen/einem”, “soll er”. (4) [_], including the result of (2), may be deleted, e.g. “haben/ können/sollen/wollen” [ha_bm]/[k_nn/[z_ln]/[v_ln], “ein” [n], “mit dem” [mId/tm], “zum” [tsm]. Subsequent rules also apply to the segment sequences resulting from (4). (5) Interconsonantal /t/ may be deleted: “sind wir” [zIn v_]. (6) Apical stop consonants are assimilated in place to follow-ing labials/dorsals, irrespective of word boundaries; apical nasals are also adjusted to preceding labials/dorsals within the same word: “haben” [ha_bm], “mit dem” [mIb/pm], “können/sind/sollen/wollen wir” [k_M/mv_]/[zIM/mv_]/[z_lM/mv_]/[v_lM/mv_]. (7) Final /l/ may be deleted, even before initial vowels of en-clitic words: “mal”, “soll(en)”, “solch”, “welch”, “will”, “wollen”. (8) Velic closure in lenis plosives before nasals may be cut out and [M/mv] integrated in a single bilabial nasal gesture [m]: “haben” [ha_m], “mit dem” [mIm];[ham_] etc.. (9) The velic closing movement for a plosive in a nasal environment is relaxed and a prosody of irregular glottal activity produced instead: “könnten” [k_ nn0 n], “sollten wir” [z_m0(m)_]. (10) A postvocalic closing movement for a nasal consonant is reduced or totally eliminated, with nasality spreading, particularly across the preceding vowel; mid to open diphthongs may be mono-phthongized: “sollen wir” [z_)V)_)][z_)_)][z_)_], “sollten wir” [z_)V)0_)]. 2. PERCEPTION PATTERNS OF FUNCTION WORDS 2.1. A Hypothesis and a New Experimental Frame Since the production patterns found in function words are an essent-ial feature of connected, especially spontaneous speech it must be assumed that they also play a fundamental role in speech percept-ion. The question thus is as to how listeners make use of phonetic parameters contained in reduced speech to restore the intended words and utterances, and what relevance should be attributed to the global prosodic features of nasalization, labialization, and glottalization, as well as to articulatory residues, over and above segmental information, for correct decoding of connected speech. To test this hypothesis we need a new type of data in our perception experiments, compared with the traditional paradigm, which uses very simple stimuli of syllable or word size, often of a nonsense word type, within a standard metalinguistic sentence frame, systematically varying acoustic parameters in speech syn-thesis. The Haskins experiments on VOT and second formant transitions are classic examples. The aim of such perception tests is to gain insight into the perceptual relevance of specific parameter values for phoneme perception in word citation forms. None of these heavy constraints apply to the phenomena at is
[1] G. G. Stokes. "J." , 1890, The New Yale Book of Quotations.
[2] Klaus J. Kohler. Complementary phonology a theoretical frame for labelling an acoustic data base of dialogues , 1994, ICSLP.