Localization of targets across saccades: Role of landmark objects

Saccadic eye movements are required to bring different parts of the visual world into the foveal region of the retina. With each saccade, the images of the objects drastically change their retinal positions—nevertheless, the visual world appears continuous and does not seem to jump. How does the visual system achieve this continuous and stable percept of the visual world, despite the gross changes of its retinal projection that occur with each saccade? The present paper argues that an important factor of this type of space constancy is formed by the reafferent information, i.e., the visual display that is found when the eyes land. Three experiments demonstrate that objects present across the saccade can serve as landmarks for postsaccadic relocalization. The basic experimental manipulation consisted of a systematic displacement of these landmark objects during the saccade. The effectiveness of the landmarks was determined by analysing to what degree they modify the perceived shift of a small saccade target that was blanked for 200 ms during and after the saccade. A first experiment studied the spatial range where objects become effective as landmarks. The data show that landmarks close to the saccade target and horizontally aligned with the target are specifically effective. The second experiment demonstrates that postsaccadic localization is normally based on relational information about relative stimulus positions transferred across the saccade. A third experiment studied the effect of a prominent background frame on transsaccadic localization; the results suggest that background structures contribute only little to transsaccadic localization.

[1]  P. Cz. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 1896 .

[2]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  L. Matin Eye Movements and Perceived Visual Direction , 1972 .

[4]  Donald M. MacKay,et al.  Visual Stability and Voluntary Eye Movements , 1973 .

[5]  E. Matin Saccadic suppression: a review and an analysis. , 1974, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[7]  Simon Heywood,et al.  Direction-specific and position-specific effects upon detection of displacements during saccadic eye movements , 1981, Vision Research.

[8]  W. Wolf,et al.  Corrective saccades: Effect of shifting the saccade goal , 1982, Vision Research.

[9]  L. Stark,et al.  Role of corollary discharge in space constancy , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  W. Wolf,et al.  The Evaluation of the Oculomotor Error Signal , 1984 .

[11]  H D Crane,et al.  Generation-V dual-Purkinje-image eyetracker. , 1985, Applied optics.

[12]  O. Grüsser,et al.  Afterimage movement during saccades in the dark , 1987, Vision Research.

[13]  H. Honda Perceptual localization of visual stimuli flashed during saccades , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  L. Stark,et al.  Ocular proprioception and efference copy in registering visual direction , 1991, Vision Research.

[15]  D. Foster,et al.  Thresholds From Psychometric Functions : Superiority of Bootstrap to Incremental and Probit Variance Estimators , 1991 .

[16]  D. E. Irwin Memory for position and identity across eye movements. , 1992 .

[17]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[18]  K. Rayner Eye movements and visual cognition : scene perception and reading , 1992 .

[19]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Spatial Memory and Integration Across Saccadic Eye Movements , 1992 .

[20]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  A localist evaluation solution for visual stability across saccades , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[21]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[22]  D. Burr,et al.  Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements , 1994, Nature.

[23]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Fourth Purkinje image signals reveal eye-lens deviations and retinal image distortions during saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[24]  C R Olson,et al.  Object-centered direction selectivity in the macaque supplementary eye field , 1995, Science.

[25]  J. Schlag,et al.  Illusory localization of stimuli flashed in the dark before saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[26]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neurons in the monkey superior colliculus predict the visual result of impending saccadic eye movements. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Paul Dassonville,et al.  The use of egocentric and exocentric location cues in saccadic programming , 1995, Vision Research.

[29]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[30]  Christopher B. Currie,et al.  Visual stability across saccades while viewing complex pictures. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[32]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement , 1996, Vision Research.

[33]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  Reference frames in saccadic targeting , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[35]  H. Deubel,et al.  Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  David E. Irwin Robert D. Gordon Eye Movements, Attention and Trans-saccadic Memory , 1998 .

[37]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Immediate post-saccadic information mediates space constancy , 1998, Vision Research.

[38]  P H Schiller,et al.  Visual representations during saccadic eye movements. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  H. Honda Modification of saccade-contingent visual mislocalization by the presence of a visual frame of reference , 1999, Vision Research.

[40]  M. Chun,et al.  Organization of visual short-term memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[41]  R. Kass,et al.  Neuronal activity in macaque supplementary eye field during planning of saccades in response to pattern and spatial cues. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  K Verfaillie,et al.  Transsaccadic memory for position and orientation of saccade source and target. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  David E. Irwin,et al.  The role of the saccade target object in the perception of a visually stable world , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[45]  Laura A. Carlson,et al.  Transsaccadic Coding of Multiple Objects and Features , 2001 .

[46]  D. Burr,et al.  Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.

[47]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Transsaccadic memory of position and form. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[48]  Jukka Hyönä,et al.  The brain's eye : neurobiological and clinical aspects of oculomotor research , 2002 .

[49]  R. Radach,et al.  Transsaccadic memory for visual object detail , 2003 .

[50]  Sven Panis,et al.  Transsaccadic integration of bystander locations , 2004 .

[51]  K. F. Austen,et al.  Oculoparalytic Illusion : Visual-Field Dependent Spatial Mislocalizations by Humans Partially Paralyzed with Curare , 2006 .