Optimizing beat synchronized running to music

The use of music and specifically tempo-matched music has been shown to affect running performance. But can we maximize the synchronization of movements to music and does maximum synchronization influence kinematics and motivation? In this study, we explore the effect of different types of music-to-movement alignment strategies on phase coherence, cadence and motivation. These strategies were compared to a control condition where the music tempo was deliberately not aligned to the running cadence. Results show that without relative phase alignment, a negative mean asynchrony (NMA) of footfall timings with respect to the beats is obtained. This means that footfalls occurred slightly before the beat and that beats were anticipated. Convergence towards this NMA or preferred relative phase angle was facilitated when the first music beat of a new song started close to the step, which means that entrainment occurred. The results also show that using tempo and phase alignment, the relative phase can be manipulated or forced in a certain angle with a high degree of accuracy. Ensuring negative angles larger than NMA (step before beat) results in increased motivation and decreasing cadence. Running in NMA or preferred relative phase angles results in a null effect on cadence. Ensuring a positive phase angle with respect to NMA results in higher motivation and higher cadence. None of the manipulations resulted in change in perceived exhaustion or a change in velocity. Results also indicate that gender plays an important role when using forced phase algorithms: effects were more pronounced for the female population than for the male population. The implementation of the proposed alignment strategies and control of beat timing while running opens possibilities optimizing the individual running cadence and motivation.

[1]  B. Repp Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  P. Terry,et al.  Effects of synchronous music on treadmill running among elite triathletes. , 2012, Journal of science and medicine in sport.

[3]  P. Terry,et al.  Development and initial validation of an instrument to assess the motivational qualities of music in exercise and sport: the Brunel Music Rating Inventory. , 1999, Journal of sports sciences.

[4]  Sean P Mullen,et al.  Measuring enjoyment of physical activity in older adults: invariance of the physical activity enjoyment scale (paces) across groups and time , 2011, International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity.

[5]  Philipp Berens,et al.  CircStat: AMATLABToolbox for Circular Statistics , 2009, Journal of Statistical Software.

[6]  A. Villringer,et al.  The Band Effect—Physically Strenuous Music Making Increases Esthetic Appreciation of Music , 2016, Front. Neurosci..

[7]  Marc Leman The Expressive Moment: How Interaction (with Music) Shapes Human Empowerment , 2016 .

[8]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  D. Geary,et al.  Psychonomic Bulletin Review , 2000 .

[10]  J. Morton,et al.  Perceptual centers (P-centers). , 1976 .

[11]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[12]  D. Moelants,et al.  Activating and Relaxing Music Entrains the Speed of Beat Synchronized Walking , 2013, PloS one.

[13]  B. Repp,et al.  Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012) , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[14]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[15]  Simon Dixon,et al.  Evaluation of the Audio Beat Tracking System BeatRoot , 2007 .

[16]  H. Håkan A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 1985 .

[17]  Hotaka Maeda,et al.  Effects of Listening to Preferential Music on Sex Differences in Endurance Running Performance , 2015, Perceptual and motor skills.

[18]  G. Madison,et al.  Musical information increases physical performance for synchronous but not asynchronous running , 2016 .

[19]  Marc Leman,et al.  Encouraging Spontaneous Synchronisation with D-Jogger, an Adaptive Music Player That Aligns Movement and Music , 2014, PloS one.

[20]  F. Mormann,et al.  Mean phase coherence as a measure for phase synchronization and its application to the EEG of epilepsy patients , 2000 .

[21]  Elizabeth J Lyons,et al.  Cultivating Engagement and Enjoyment in Exergames Using Feedback, Challenge, and Rewards. , 2015, Games for health journal.

[22]  Jorge A. Santos,et al.  The effect of instruction to synchronize over step frequency while walking with auditory cues on a treadmill. , 2014, Human movement science.

[23]  J. van der Kamp,et al.  The Power of Auditory-Motor Synchronization in Sports: Enhancing Running Performance by Coupling Cadence with the Right Beats , 2013, PloS one.

[24]  Daniel T Bishop,et al.  Psychophysiological effects of synchronous versus asynchronous music during cycling. , 2014, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[25]  M. Leman,et al.  3Mo: A Model for Music-Based Biofeedback , 2016, Front. Neurosci..

[26]  Jason M Haberman,et al.  Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Deborah Kendzierski,et al.  Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale: Two Validation Studies , 1991 .

[28]  Edith Van Dyck,et al.  Spontaneous Velocity Effect of Musical Expression on Self-Paced Walking , 2016, PloS one.

[29]  Marc Leman,et al.  Musical agency reduces perceived exertion during strenuous physical performance , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Nicholas I. Fisher,et al.  Statistical Analysis of Circular Data , 1993 .

[31]  K. Mardia A Multi‐Sample Uniform Scores Test on a Circle and its Parametric Competitor , 1972 .

[32]  Michael J. Hove,et al.  Interactive Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation Reinstates Natural 1/f Timing in Gait of Parkinson's Patients , 2012, PloS one.

[33]  Algorithms to measure audio programme loudness and true-peak audio level , 2011 .

[34]  Andrew M Lane,et al.  Redesign and initial validation of an instrument to assess the motivational qualities of music in exercise: The Brunel Music Rating Inventory-2 , 2006, Journal of sports sciences.

[35]  Lukáš Režný,et al.  Tempo and walking speed with music in the urban context , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[36]  Costas I. Karageorghis,et al.  Music in the exercise domain: a review and synthesis (Part I) , 2011, International review of sport and exercise psychology.

[37]  Anne Corcos Being enjoyably challenged is the key to an enjoyable gaming experience: an experimental approach in a first-person shooter game , 2018, Socioaffective neuroscience & psychology.

[38]  G. Borg Borg's Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales , 1998 .

[39]  D. Redelmeier,et al.  Synchronized personalized music audio-playlists to improve adherence to physical activity among patients participating in a structured exercise program: a proof-of-principle feasibility study , 2015, Sports Medicine - Open.

[40]  M.R. Popovic,et al.  A reliable gait phase detection system , 2001, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[41]  Jane E. Clark,et al.  Timing at peak force may be the hidden target controlled in continuation and synchronization tapping , 2017, Experimental Brain Research.