Practices and pitfalls in inferring neural representations

A key challenge for cognitive neuroscience is deciphering the representational schemes of the brain. Stimulus-feature-based encoding models are becoming increasingly popular for inferring the dimensions of neural representational spaces from stimulus-feature spaces. We argue that such inferences are not always valid because successful prediction can occur even if the two representational spaces use different, but correlated, representational schemes. We support this claim with three simulations in which we achieved high prediction accuracy despite systematic differences in the geometries and dimensions of the underlying representations. Detailed analysis of the encoding models' predictions showed systematic deviations from ground-truth, indicating that high prediction accuracy is insufficient for making representational inferences. This fallacy applies to the prediction of actual neural patterns from stimulus-feature spaces and we urge caution in inferring the nature of the neural code from such methods. We discuss ways to overcome these inferential limitations, including model comparison, absolute model performance, visualization techniques and attentional modulation.

[1]  Michael A. Casey,et al.  Population Codes Representing Musical Timbre for High-Level fMRI Categorization of Music Genres , 2011, MLINI.

[2]  J. Fodor,et al.  Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis , 1988, Cognition.

[3]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Heteromodal Cortical Areas Encode Sensory-Motor Features of Word Meaning , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Decoding Representations of Scenes in the Medial Temporal Lobes , 2011, Hippocampus.

[5]  M Congedo,et al.  A review of classification algorithms for EEG-based brain–computer interfaces , 2007, Journal of neural engineering.

[6]  Stefan Winkler,et al.  Color Space Conversions , 2013 .

[7]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Measuring neural representations with fMRI: practices and pitfalls , 2013, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Michael S. Pratte,et al.  Decoding patterns of human brain activity. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.

[9]  Colin Klein,et al.  Decoding the Brain: Neural Representation and the Limits of Multivariate Pattern Analysis in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2017 .

[10]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Importance of Falsification in Computational Cognitive Modeling , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Jack L. Gallant,et al.  Encoding and decoding in fMRI , 2011, NeuroImage.

[12]  R. Henson What can Functional Neuroimaging Tell the Experimental Psychologist? , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A computational model of semantic memory impairment: modality specificity and emergent category specificity. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  J Brendan Ritchie,et al.  Decoding the Brain: Neural Representation and the Limits of Multivariate Pattern Analysis in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2017, bioRxiv.

[15]  Simon Kirby,et al.  How arbitrary is language? , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[16]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  Alexander G. Huth,et al.  Attention During Natural Vision Warps Semantic Representation Across the Human Brain , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  Sean M. Polyn,et al.  Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  Giancarlo Valente,et al.  Brain-Based Translation: fMRI Decoding of Spoken Words in Bilinguals Reveals Language-Independent Semantic Representations in Anterior Temporal Lobe , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  David W. Sutterer,et al.  The topography of alpha-band activity tracks the content of spatial working memory. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  B. Love,et al.  Occipitotemporal Representations Reflect Individual Differences in Conceptual Knowledge , 2018, bioRxiv.

[22]  Essa Yacoub,et al.  Encoding of Natural Sounds at Multiple Spectral and Temporal Resolutions in the Human Auditory Cortex , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[23]  A. Ishai,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.

[24]  J. S. Guntupalli,et al.  Decoding neural representational spaces using multivariate pattern analysis. , 2014, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  Thomas A Carlson,et al.  Orientation Decoding in Human Visual Cortex: New Insights from an Unbiased Perspective , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Chris I. Baker,et al.  Deconstructing multivariate decoding for the study of brain function , 2017, NeuroImage.

[27]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Reconstructing Perceived and Retrieved Faces from Activity Patterns in Lateral Parietal Cortex , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  S. Thompson-Schill,et al.  Creating Concepts from Converging Features in Human Cortex. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  R. Poldrack Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Jack L. Gallant,et al.  A Continuous Semantic Space Describes the Representation of Thousands of Object and Action Categories across the Human Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[31]  A. Dale,et al.  Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. , 1998, Science.

[32]  N. Sigala,et al.  Visual categorization shapes feature selectivity in the primate temporal cortex , 2002, Nature.

[33]  N. Kriegeskorte,et al.  Author ' s personal copy Representational geometry : integrating cognition , computation , and the brain , 2013 .

[34]  Massimo Poesio,et al.  Reading visually embodied meaning from the brain: Visually grounded computational models decode visual-object mental imagery induced by written text , 2015, NeuroImage.

[35]  Thomas Naselaris,et al.  Resolving Ambiguities of MVPA Using Explicit Models of Representation , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Samuel A. Nastase,et al.  Attention Selectively Reshapes the Geometry of Distributed Semantic Representation , 2016, bioRxiv.

[37]  Tom Michael Mitchell,et al.  Predicting Human Brain Activity Associated with the Meanings of Nouns , 2008, Science.

[38]  F. Tong,et al.  Decoding the visual and subjective contents of the human brain , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Marc N. Coutanche,et al.  Distinguishing Multi-voxel Patterns and Mean Activation: Why, How, and What Does It Tell Us? a Question of Spatial Frequency , 2022 .

[40]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  The burden of embodied cognition. , 2015, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[41]  R. Poldrack Inferring Mental States from Neuroimaging Data: From Reverse Inference to Large-Scale Decoding , 2011, Neuron.

[42]  J. Gallant,et al.  Identifying natural images from human brain activity , 2008, Nature.

[43]  D. Heeger,et al.  Decoding and Reconstructing Color from Responses in Human Visual Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Mamiko Niwa,et al.  Task Engagement Selectively Modulates Neural Correlations in Primary Auditory Cortex , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Jeremy Freeman,et al.  Orientation Decoding Depends on Maps, Not Columns , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  M. Weiner,et al.  Neuroimaging markers for the prediction and early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease dementia , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.