Impact of Different Speech Types on Listening Effort

Listeners are exposed to different types of speech in everyday life, from natural speech to speech that has undergone modifications or has been generated synthetically. While many studies have focused on measuring the intelligibility of these distinct speech types, their impact on listening effort is not known. The current study combined an objective measure of intelligibility, a physiological measure of listening effort (pupil size) and listeners’ subjective judgements, to examine the impact of four speech types: plain (natural) speech, speech produced in noise (Lombard speech), speech enhanced to promote intelligibility, and synthetic speech. For each speech type, listeners responded to sentences presented in one of three levels of speech-shaped noise. Subjective effort ratings and intelligibility scores showed an inverse ranking across speech types, with synthetic speech being the most demanding and enhanced speech the least. Pupil size measures indicated an increase in listening effort with decreasing signal-to-noise ratio for all speech types apart from synthetic speech, which required significantly more effort at the most favourable noise level. Naturally and artificially modified speech were less effortful than plain speech at the more adverse noise levels. These outcomes indicate a clear impact of speech type on the cognitive demands required for comprehension.

[1]  Yannis Stylianou,et al.  Speech-in-noise intelligibility improvement based on spectral shaping and dynamic range compression , 2012, INTERSPEECH.

[2]  A. Zekveld,et al.  Pupil Response as an Indication of Effortful Listening: The Influence of Sentence Intelligibility , 2010, Ear and hearing.

[3]  A. Zekveld,et al.  Cognitive processing load across a wide range of listening conditions: insights from pupillometry. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[4]  Yannis Stylianou,et al.  Evaluating the intelligibility benefit of speech modifications in known noise conditions , 2013, Speech Commun..

[5]  Carol L Mackersie,et al.  Subjective and psychophysiological indexes of listening effort in a competing-talker task. , 2011, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.

[6]  G. Studebaker A "rationalized" arcsine transform. , 1985, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[7]  Thomas Lunner,et al.  Impact of stimulus-related factors and hearing impairment on listening effort as indicated by pupil dilation , 2017, Hearing Research.

[8]  Torsten Dau,et al.  Impact of Background Noise and Sentence Complexity on Processing Demands during Sentence Comprehension , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[9]  Cassia Valentini-Botinhao,et al.  Intelligibility-enhancing speech modifications: the hurricane challenge , 2020, INTERSPEECH.

[10]  A. Stewart,et al.  Listening effort and fatigue: What exactly are we measuring? A British Society of Audiology Cognition in Hearing Special Interest Group ‘white paper’ , 2014, International journal of audiology.

[11]  Sridhar Kalluri,et al.  Objective measures of listening effort: effects of background noise and noise reduction. , 2009, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[13]  R. H. Bernacki,et al.  Effects of noise on speech production: acoustic and perceptual analyses. , 1988, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Speech recognition in adverse conditions: A review , 2012 .

[15]  Heiga Zen,et al.  Robust Speaker-Adaptive HMM-Based Text-to-Speech Synthesis , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Audio, Speech, and Language Processing.

[16]  P. Brockhoff,et al.  Delta-tilde interpretation of standard linear mixed model results , 2016 .

[17]  A. Zekveld,et al.  Pupil Dilation Uncovers Extra Listening Effort in the Presence of a Single-Talker Masker , 2012, Ear and hearing.

[18]  A. Zekveld,et al.  The influence of informational masking on speech perception and pupil response in adults with hearing impairment. , 2014, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  J. Peelle Listening Effort: How the Cognitive Consequences of Acoustic Challenge Are Reflected in Brain and Behavior , 2017, Ear and hearing.

[20]  Anita Wagner,et al.  How hard can it be to ignore the pan in panda? Effort of lexical competition as measured in pupil dilation , 2015, ICPhS.

[21]  Yu-Hsiang Wu,et al.  Psychometric Functions of Dual-Task Paradigms for Measuring Listening Effort , 2016, Ear and hearing.

[22]  Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham,et al.  The pupil response reveals increased listening effort when it is difficult to focus attention , 2015, Hearing Research.

[23]  Matthew B. Winn,et al.  The Impact of Auditory Spectral Resolution on Listening Effort Revealed by Pupil Dilation , 2015, Ear and hearing.

[24]  IEEE Recommended Practice for Speech Quality Measurements , 1969, IEEE Transactions on Audio and Electroacoustics.