Numerical estimation strategies are correlated with math ability in school-aged children

Abstract Various laboratory-based numerical cognition measures are often found to be linked with standardized math scores, yet the exact nature of these links over development remains in question. In this study, we investigated how 0–100 number line estimation performance and dot numerosity discrimination explained variance in 6- to 8-year-olds’ standardized math ability scores, and we assessed links between tasks. Our primary goal was to assess the link between estimation performance and math ability from a theoretical perspective that models number line estimation as an instance of proportion estimation. As age increased, children’s proportion-based strategies for number line estimation became more sophisticated and estimation bias decreased. Children who spontaneously inferred and used a central reference point to guide their proportion estimates scored higher on the math ability measure (TEMA-3), demonstrating a relation between more advanced proportion estimation strategies and higher math abilities. Individual differences in estimation bias also made a lesser contribution to explaining variance in children’s math ability. Dot discrimination accuracy uniquely contributed to children’s math ability. Number line estimation performance and dot discrimination accuracy were not associated, consistent with prior research suggesting that number line estimation does not directly map onto mental numerical magnitude representations. Educational implications and future directions are discussed.

[1]  M. Ashcraft,et al.  Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in children. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  A. Leung,et al.  Number Line Estimation Predicts Mathematical Skills: Difference in Grades 2 and 4 , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[3]  R. Siegler,et al.  The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.

[4]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[5]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  Unbounding the mental number line—new evidence on children's spatial representation of numbers , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[6]  D. Cohen,et al.  Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  On the Relation between the Mental Number Line and Arithmetic Competencies , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[10]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  Psychophysics of Numerical Representation , 2011 .

[11]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[12]  J. Halberda,et al.  Approximate number and approximate time discrimination each correlate with school math abilities in young children. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[13]  David Landy,et al.  Estimating Large Numbers , 2013, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  H. Barth,et al.  Spontaneous partitioning and proportion estimation in children's numerical judgments. , 2019, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[15]  Neil Marlow,et al.  Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control, Not Non-Verbal Number Acuity, Correlate with Mathematics Achievement , 2013, PloS one.

[16]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  Children's Understanding of the Natural Numbers' Structure , 2018, Cogn. Sci..

[17]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: differentiating between scale and bias. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The representation of numerical magnitude , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[19]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[20]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Inhibitory control may not explain the link between approximation and math abilities in kindergarteners from middle class families , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[21]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[22]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. , 2011, Developmental science.

[23]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Comment on "Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures" , 2009, Science.

[24]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Playing linear number board games—but not circular ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. , 2009 .

[25]  D. Peeters,et al.  Benchmark‐based strategies in whole number line estimation , 2017, British journal of psychology.

[26]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli , 2011, Behavior research methods.

[27]  Elizabeth A. Gunderson,et al.  The Number Line Is a Critical Spatial-Numerical Representation: Evidence From a Fraction Intervention , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[28]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  H. Barth,et al.  Intuitive proportion judgment in number-line estimation: Converging evidence from multiple tasks. , 2017, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[30]  Gulen Baran,et al.  A Study on the Effect of Mathematics Teaching Provided through Drama on the Mathematics Ability of Six-Year-Old Children. , 2009 .

[31]  Emmy Defever,et al.  The Approximate Number System is not Predictive for Symbolic Number Processing in Kindergarteners , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. , 2008, Child development.

[33]  Susan Carey,et al.  One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.

[34]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Model Selection and Multimodel Inference , 2003 .

[36]  D. Ansari,et al.  What is the precise role of cognitive control in the development of a sense of number? , 2017, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[37]  Robert S Siegler,et al.  The powers of noise-fitting: reply to Barth and Paladino. , 2011, Developmental science.

[38]  Ariel Starr,et al.  Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children's numerical knowledge through playing number board games. , 2008, Child development.

[40]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  Children's cognitive representation of the mathematical number line. , 2014, Developmental science.

[41]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: what underlies mathematics achievement? , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[42]  Lance J Rips,et al.  How many is a zillion? Sources of number distortion. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  H. Barth,et al.  How feedback improves children’s numerical estimation , 2016, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  Karin Landerl,et al.  Typical and atypical development of basic numerical skills in elementary school. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[45]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[46]  H. Barth,et al.  Digit identity influences numerical estimation in children and adults. , 2018, Developmental science.

[47]  Barbara J. Juhasz,et al.  Adults’ number-line estimation strategies: Evidence from eye movements , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  J. Schweitzer,et al.  Owner reports of attention, activity, and impulsivity in dogs: a replication study , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[49]  R. Prather Neural coding partially accounts for the relationship between children’s number-line estimation and number comparison performance , 2018 .

[50]  H. Barth,et al.  Judgments of discrete and continuous quantity: An illusory Stroop effect , 2008, Cognition.

[51]  R. Teghtsoonian The standard model for perceived magnitude: a framework for (almost) everything known about it. , 2012, The American journal of psychology.

[52]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[53]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Associations of Number Line Estimation With Mathematical Competence: A Meta-analysis. , 2018, Child development.

[54]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Changing the precision of preschoolers' approximate number system representations changes their symbolic math performance. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[55]  Fruzsina Soltész,et al.  Relationships between magnitude representation, counting and memory in 4- to 7-year-old children: A developmental study , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[56]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Measuring the Approximate Number System , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[57]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.

[58]  H. Barth,et al.  Spatial estimation: a non-Bayesian alternative. , 2015, Developmental science.

[59]  Russell Gersten,et al.  Developmental predictors of fraction concepts and procedures. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[60]  B. Reynvoet,et al.  Enhancing arithmetic in pre-schoolers with comparison or number line estimation training: Does it matter? ☆ , 2016 .

[61]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  The Logarithmic-To-Linear Shift: One Learning Sequence, Many Tasks, Many Time Scales , 2009 .

[62]  Ariel Starr,et al.  The contributions of numerical acuity and non-numerical stimulus features to the development of the number sense and symbolic math achievement , 2017, Cognition.

[63]  Emily Slusser,et al.  COMMENTARY A sense of proportion: commentary on Opfer, Siegler and Young , 2011 .

[64]  A. Desoete,et al.  Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study. , 2012, The British journal of educational psychology.

[65]  Percival G. Matthews,et al.  Knowledge on the line: Manipulating beliefs about the magnitudes of symbolic numbers affects the linearity of line estimation tasks , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[66]  A. Petitto,et al.  Development of Numberline and Measurement Concepts , 1990 .

[67]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Individual differences in nonverbal number discrimination correlate with event-related potentials and measures of probabilistic reasoning , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  A. Henik,et al.  The contribution of fish studies to the “number sense” debate , 2016, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[69]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  Numerical landmarks are useful--except when they're not. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[70]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[71]  David C. Geary,et al.  Development of Number Line Representations in Children With Mathematical Learning Disability , 2008, Developmental neuropsychology.

[72]  Lisa K Fazio,et al.  Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[73]  L. Thompson,et al.  Number Sense and Mathematics: Which, When and How? , 2017, Developmental psychology.

[74]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Indexing the approximate number system. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[75]  H. Barth,et al.  Developmental change in numerical estimation. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[76]  Barbara W Sarnecka,et al.  Children's number-line estimation shows development of measurement skills (not number representations). , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[77]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Representational change and children’s numerical estimation , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.

[78]  Gavin R. Price,et al.  Numerical predictors of arithmetic success in grades 1-6. , 2014, Developmental science.

[79]  I. Spence Visual psychophysics of simple graphical elements. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[80]  Sara Cordes,et al.  123s and ABCs: developmental shifts in logarithmic-to-linear responding reflect fluency with sequence values. , 2014, Developmental science.

[81]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Nonsymbolic number and cumulative area representations contribute shared and unique variance to symbolic math competence , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[82]  P. Quinlan,et al.  The log–linear response function of the bounded number-line task is unrelated to the psychological representation of quantity , 2018, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[83]  Hilary C Barth,et al.  The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.

[84]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  How 15 hundred is like 15 cherries: effect of progressive alignment on representational changes in numerical cognition. , 2010, Child development.

[85]  C. Hulme,et al.  The relationship between numerosity discrimination and arithmetic skill reflects the approximate number system and cannot be explained by inhibitory control. , 2019, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[86]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Does numerical processing uniquely predict first graders’ future development of single-digit arithmetic? , 2015 .

[87]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Mental number line, number line estimation, and mathematical achievement : Their interrelations in grades 5 and 6 , 2009 .

[88]  R. Newman,et al.  Children's Numerical Estimation: Flexibility in the Use of Counting. , 1984 .

[89]  Nicole M. McNeil,et al.  ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: contributions of inhibitory control. , 2013, Developmental science.

[90]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[91]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Numerical approximation abilities correlate with and predict informal but not formal mathematics abilities. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[92]  George K. Georgiou,et al.  The Role of Approximate Number System in Different Mathematics Skills Across Grades , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[93]  Hilary Barth,et al.  Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[94]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Training the Approximate Number System Improves Math Proficiency , 2013, Psychological science.

[95]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  A validation of eye movements as a measure of elementary school children's developing number sense , 2008 .

[96]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  Free versus anchored numerical estimation: A unified approach , 2016, Cognition.

[97]  Dominique Peeters,et al.  Children’s use of number line estimation strategies , 2016 .

[98]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  The precision of mapping between number words and the approximate number system predicts children's formal math abilities. , 2016, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[99]  Brian P. Dyre,et al.  Understanding bias in proportion production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[100]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). , 2011, Child development.

[101]  Brian P. Dyre,et al.  Bias in proportion judgments: the cyclical power model. , 2000, Psychological review.

[102]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[103]  Strategies in unbounded number line estimation? Evidence from eye-tracking , 2015, Cognitive Processing.

[104]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  Intuitive Numbers Guide Decisions , 2008, Judgment and Decision Making.

[105]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling , 2010, Cognition.

[106]  E. Kroesbergen,et al.  Longitudinal development of number line estimation and mathematics performance in primary school children. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[107]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Ratio scales and category scales for a dozen perceptual continua. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[108]  L. Feigenson,et al.  Approximate number sense correlates with math performance in gifted adolescents. , 2017, Acta psychologica.

[109]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[110]  Julie L. Booth,et al.  Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[111]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Domain-specific and domain-general changes in children's development of number comparison. , 2008, Developmental science.