Decomposing Decision Components in the Stop-signal Task: A Model-based Approach to Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control

The stop-signal task, in which participants must inhibit prepotent responses, has been used to identify neural systems that vary with individual differences in inhibitory control. To explore how these differences relate to other aspects of decision making, a drift-diffusion model of simple decisions was fitted to stop-signal task data from go trials to extract measures of caution, motor execution time, and stimulus processing speed for each of 123 participants. These values were used to probe fMRI data to explore individual differences in neural activation. Faster processing of the go stimulus correlated with greater activation in the right frontal pole for both go and stop trials. On stop trials, stimulus processing speed also correlated with regions implicated in inhibitory control, including the right inferior frontal gyrus, medial frontal gyrus, and BG. Individual differences in motor execution time correlated with activation of the right parietal cortex. These findings suggest a robust relationship between the speed of stimulus processing and inhibitory processing at the neural level. This model-based approach provides novel insight into the interrelationships among decision components involved in inhibitory control and raises interesting questions about strategic adjustments in performance and inhibitory deficits associated with psychopathology.

[1]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[2]  B. Forstmann,et al.  Perceptual decision neurosciences – A model-based review , 2014, Neuroscience.

[3]  David C. Van Essen,et al.  A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[4]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..

[5]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Responding with Restraint: What Are the Neurocognitive Mechanisms? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  G. Logan,et al.  Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[7]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[9]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Using diffusion models to understand clinical disorders. , 2010, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[10]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[11]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[12]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Risk prediction and aversion by anterior cingulate cortex , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  A Population-Average, Landmark- and Surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. , 2005, NeuroImage.

[14]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  From model-based perceptual decision-making to spatial interference control , 2015, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[15]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  C. White,et al.  Perceptual Criteria in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  G. Logan,et al.  Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL , 2004, NeuroImage.

[19]  Lourens J. Waldorp,et al.  Visual Information Shapes the Dynamics of Corticobasal Ganglia Pathways during Response Selection and Inhibition , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Monterosso,et al.  Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. , 2005, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[21]  Nitin Tandon,et al.  Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[22]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Engagement of large-scale networks is related to individual differences in inhibitory control , 2010, NeuroImage.

[23]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[24]  Eliza Congdon,et al.  Neural activation during response inhibition in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Preliminary findings on the effects of medication and symptom severity , 2014, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[25]  Russell Schachar,et al.  Inhibitory control and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task , 2010, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[26]  Carsten Nicolas Boehler,et al.  Pinning down response inhibition in the brain — Conjunction analyses of the Stop-signal task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[27]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The Neural Substrate of Prior Information in Perceptual Decision Making: A Model-Based Analysis , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[28]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[29]  Qiang Wang,et al.  The Role of the Frontal and Parietal Cortex in Proactive and Reactive Inhibitory Control: A Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Study , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Camille F. Chavan,et al.  Differential patterns of functional and structural plasticity within and between inferior frontal gyri support training‐induced improvements in inhibitory control proficiency , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[31]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[32]  L. Spierer,et al.  Enhancing frontal top-down inhibitory control with Go/NoGo training , 2015, Brain Structure and Function.

[33]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Cortical Dynamics of Sensorimotor Integration during Grasp Planning , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Jenkinson Non-linear registration aka Spatial normalisation , 2007 .

[35]  Eliza Congdon,et al.  Measurement and Reliability of Response Inhibition , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[36]  Lucas Spierer,et al.  Sustained enhancements in inhibitory control depend primarily on the reinforcement of fronto-basal anatomical connectivity , 2015, Brain Structure and Function.

[37]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  Shuhei Yamaguchi,et al.  The Key Locus of Common Response Inhibition Network for No-go and Stop Signals , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[40]  M. Fillmore,et al.  Polydrug abusers display impaired discrimination-reversal learning in a model of behavioural control , 2006, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[41]  Michael J. Kofler,et al.  Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Behavioral Inhibition: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2007, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[42]  Dc Washington Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. , 1994 .

[43]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  Angela J. Yu,et al.  Statistical learning and adaptive decision-making underlie human response time variability in inhibitory control , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[45]  R. Bogacz,et al.  The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.

[46]  M. First,et al.  Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders : SCID-I: clinical version : administration booklet , 1996 .

[47]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[49]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Cortico-striatal connections predict control over speed and accuracy in perceptual decision making , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  M. Jenkinson,et al.  Non-linear optimisation FMRIB Technial Report TR 07 JA 1 , 2007 .

[51]  Janet B W Williams,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[52]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[54]  Christopher J. Cannistraci,et al.  Sensorimotor‐independent prefrontal activity during response inhibition , 2014, Human brain mapping.