Dynamic Integration

Positive self- and emotional development is often measured by optimization of happiness, but a second aspect of positive development—the ability to tolerate tension and negativity in the interest of maintaining objective representations—needs to be integrated with this hedonic emphasis. The integration of these two aspects, optimization and differentiation, reflects a dynamic balance. Such integration is possible when emotional activation or arousal is moderate, but is impaired at very high levels of activation. From youth to middle adulthood, the capacity for integration increases, but later in life, limitations or poor regulation strategies foster compensatory processes that compromise integration.

[1]  Jonathan W. Schooler,et al.  Verbalization produces a transfer inappropriate processing shift. , 2002 .

[2]  Paul B. Baltes,et al.  Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences , 1990 .

[3]  P. Baltes,et al.  Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. , 1990 .

[4]  G. Labouvie-vief Psyche and Eros: Mind and Gender in the Life Course , 1994 .

[5]  J. Halberstadt,et al.  Effects of Reasons Analysis on the Accuracy of Predicting Basketball Games1 , 1999 .

[6]  G. Labouvie-vief,et al.  Speaking about feelings: conceptions of emotion across the life span. , 1989, Psychology and aging.

[7]  G. Labouvie-vief,et al.  Affect optimization and affect complexity: modes and styles of regulation in adulthood. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[8]  G. Labouvie-vief,et al.  Dynamic integration: Affect optimization and differentiation in development. , 2004 .

[9]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: dynamics of willpower. , 1999, Psychological review.

[10]  E. Deci,et al.  On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.

[11]  P. Ekman,et al.  Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  D. Paulhus,et al.  Arousal and evaluative extremity in social judgments: a dynamic complexity model , 1994 .

[13]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  A Continuum of Impression Formation, from Category-Based to Individuating Processes: Influences of Information and Motivation on Attention and Interpretation , 1990 .

[14]  R. Hastie,et al.  Person memory: Personality traits as organizing principles in memory for behaviors. , 1979 .

[15]  S. Srivastava,et al.  Three paths of adult development: conservers, seekers, and achievers. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  L. Hearnshaw,et al.  The comparative psychology of mental development , 1966 .

[17]  J. Schooler,et al.  The Misremembrance of Wines Past: Verbal and Perceptual Expertise Differentially Mediate Verbal Overshadowing of Taste Memory , 1996 .

[18]  Delroy L. Paulhus,et al.  Self-Deception: An Adaptive Mechanism? , 1988 .

[19]  M. Farah,et al.  Parts and Wholes in Face Recognition , 1993, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  J. Piaget,et al.  Intelligence and Affectivity: Their Relationship During Child Development , 1981 .

[21]  Erik H. Erikson,et al.  The Life Cycle Completed , 1982 .