The Social Impact of Self-Regulated Creativity on the Evolution of Simple versus Complex Creative Ideas

Since creative individuals invest in unproven ideas at the expense of propagating proven ones, excess creativity can be detrimental to society; moreover, some individuals benefit from creativity without being creative themselves by copying creators. This paper builds on previous studies of how societies evolve faster by tempering the novelty-generating effects of creativity with the novelty-preserving effects of imitation. It was hypothesized that (1) this balance can be achieved through self-regulation (SR) of creativity, by varying how creative one is according to the value of one’ creative outputs, and (2) that the social benefit of SR is affected by the openness of the space of possible ideas. These hypotheses were tested using EVOC, an agent-based model of cultural evolution in which each agent self-regulated its invention-to-imitation ratio as a function of the fitness of its inventions. We compared SR to non-SR societies, and compared societies in which the space of possible ideas was open-ended because agents could chain simple ideas into complex ones, to societies without chaining, for which the space of possible ideas was fixed. Agents in SR societies gradually segregated into creators and imitators, and changes in diversity were rapider and more pronounced than non-SR. The mean fitness of ideas was higher in SR than non-SR societies, but this difference was temporary without chaining whereas it was permanent with chaining. We discuss limitations of the model and possible social implications of the results.

[1]  Zoltán Rihmer,et al.  Creativity and mental illness , 1995, American Journal of Psychiatry.

[2]  A. Furnham,et al.  Creativity, Intelligence, and Personality: A Critical Review of the Scattered Literature , 2006, Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs.

[3]  Steve R. DiPaola,et al.  How Did Humans Become So Creative? A Computational Approach , 2013, ICCC.

[4]  John H. Holland,et al.  Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Systems: An Introductory Analysis with Applications to Biology, Control, and Artificial Intelligence , 1992 .

[5]  M. Field Defying the Crowd: Cultivating Creativity in a Culture of Conformity , 1996 .

[6]  Charles E. Taylor,et al.  Artificial Life II , 1991 .

[7]  Maolin Ye,et al.  The Dark Side of Creativity , 2015 .

[8]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  How Learning Can Guide Evolution , 1996, Complex Syst..

[9]  L. Pil The Rise of the Creative Class , 2004 .

[10]  Frank J. Sulloway Born to Rebel , 1996 .

[11]  K. Jamison,et al.  Touched with fire : manic-depressive illness and the artistic temperament , 1993 .

[12]  Jean E. Pretz,et al.  Individual differences in creativity: personality, story writing, and hobbies , 2001 .

[13]  Liane Gabora,et al.  A Computational Model of Two Cognitive Transitions Underlying Cultural Evolution , 2013, CogSci.

[14]  Melanie Keller,et al.  The Courage To Create , 2016 .

[15]  A. Flaherty Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive , 2005, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[16]  Paul G. Higgs The mimetic transition: a simulation study of the evolution of learning by imitation , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  Seana Moran,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity: The Roles of Creativity in Society , 2010 .

[18]  Albert Rothenberg,et al.  Creativity, mental health, and alcoholism , 1990 .

[19]  F Fagan Alcohol and the writer. , 1973, JAMA.

[20]  Anna Craft,et al.  Creativity in Schools : Tensions and Dilemmas , 2005 .

[21]  Edwin C. Selby,et al.  Assessing Creativity: A Guide for Educators. , 2002 .

[22]  N. Andreasen Creativity and mental illness: prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives. , 1987, The American journal of psychiatry.

[23]  M. Donald Origins of the modern mind , 1991 .

[24]  Leonard Carmichael,et al.  Creativity and Its Cultivation , 1961 .

[25]  Teresa M. Amabile,et al.  ? + ? = creativity. , 2018, Public health nursing.

[26]  J. Kaufman,et al.  THE COST OF THE MUSE: POETS DIE YOUNG , 2003, Death studies.

[27]  Liane Gabora,et al.  Society Functions Best with an Intermediate Level of Creativity , 2013, CogSci.

[28]  Jiliang Shen,et al.  A Model of Chinese Adolescents' Creative Personality , 2010 .

[29]  Lisa Troyer,et al.  Conflict and Creativity in Groups , 2009 .

[30]  Christina L. Scott,et al.  Teachers' Biases Toward Creative Children , 1999 .

[31]  Brian Hazlehurst,et al.  Learning in the Cultural Process , 2000 .

[32]  H. Eysenck Creativity and Personality: Suggestions for a Theory , 1993 .

[33]  Liane Gabora,et al.  How Creative Should Creators Be To Optimize the Evolution of Ideas? A Computational Model , 2009, DCM.

[34]  L. Gabora Meme and Variations: A Computational Model of Cultural Evolution , 1995 .

[35]  Michael L. Best,et al.  How Culture Can Guide Evolution: An Inquiry into Gene/Meme Enhancement and Opposition , 1999, Adapt. Behav..

[36]  Peter Bryant,et al.  The price of greatness , 1996, Nature.

[37]  R. Florida The Rise of the Creative Class , 2002 .

[38]  Christine A. Readdick,et al.  The Relation Between Aggression and Creativity Among Second Graders , 2006 .

[39]  E. Paul Torrance,et al.  Guiding creative talent , 1962 .

[40]  Abraham H. Maslow,et al.  Creativity in self-actualizing people. , 1962 .

[41]  Dan Ariely,et al.  The Dark Side of Creativity : Original Thinkers Can Be More Dishonest , 2013 .

[42]  Kirsty Kitto,et al.  Concept Combination and the Origins of Complex Cognition , 2013, ArXiv.

[43]  Diederik Aerts,et al.  A model of the emergence and evolution of integrated worldviews , 2009, 1001.1399.

[44]  C. Rogers Toward a theory of creativity. , 1954 .